• No results found

This chapter outlines two kinds of people engaged in research on the anthropology of religion: sociologically minded researchers basing their statements on the scientific method, and anthropologists who find them- selves able, even if only for a time, to adopt the religion, ritual, or healing methods of another. The scientists have a certain style and attitude embedded in their presentations, and these show up in the distancing that the method requires, suggest the kind of assumptions brought into the study of religion that derive from metropolitan psychology, and reveal the analytic tools employed to dissect ritual and religious experience. Social scientists have been digging into the working integument of it, failing to find a “soul,” and saying the religious systems are “constructed.” Then there are areas in which hesitations occur in field participation, with- drawals in order to create a safety zone against too much involvement. Conversely, I will relate some of the experiences that the full participators have recorded when sharing the ritual and religion of their field friends, and I attempt to estimate where faulty assumptions have been made on that side. Finally I ask, what are the implications of translogical experi- ences? How far ought we to go? If there is a world of spirits, a land of bliss, can it be explored anthropologically? I have from time to time attempted to do so, foolhardy though it may appear to be. I have been learning about Iñupiat hunting perceptions, healing, and near-death and ghost experi- ences. I will give certain experiences of my own as illustrations of the sub- jective effect.

What actually causes the use of the wrong tools, causes the hesitations? This is usually the fear of religious emotion, perhaps meaning a shyness,

an embarrassment with what one has been taught is a phony God. And the feeling is almost sexually unpleasant, related to the dread of funda- mentalism, in which one is snatched up and locked into a strict belief and morality system that one has, through Durkheim and Foucault, learned to hate. But the sense of “something else” may overtake us when in a really caring state or state of urgent necessity. Even so, many of us could not pos- sibly overcome that feeling of a tabooed area enough to enter the forbid- den territory.

But why should we have to be inquiring into the emotional states of researchers? What kind of people are anthropologists supposed to be? Naturally that all depends on the area of research; for agricultural anthro- pologists to identify rice varieties they will not need to be personally familiar with different levels of religious emotion. But send a young woman anthropologist into a Balinese temple for the first time, and if she does not let out a sigh of awe as Margaret Wiener did, maybe she is in the wrong branch of the discipline. One needs to react. No one today dis- agrees that this is appropriate—when it is put like this. The taboos of anthropology are breaking fast, just as the taboos of most of the mainline churches are breaking.

It is about time to look back and see how far we have come.

To gauge the changes, I first take an example from the psychiatric anthropologists Edward Weyer (1932) and Seymour Parker (1977). I quote:

During the cold winter nights the Eskimos frequently crowd together in one of the snow huts to watch the hysterical-like behavior of the shaman who is believed to be possessed by a foreign spirit with whom he is doing battle. This histrionic exhi- bition is contagious and the awestruck onlookers are often thrown into a similar state of ecstasy. (Weyer 1932:437)

Thus, comments Parker,

the Eskimos frequently witness and partake in socially sanctioned (phenotypical) hysterical-like behavior designed to achieve various gratifications, control super- natural forces, and alleviate misfortune. Such experiences could provide a model for pathological behavioral reactions to personal psychological crises. (Parker 1977:356)

I am not joking; this was generally the tone of the Freudian anthropolo- gists of the era. In the old days, we students would have trembled at and respected what these great ones said, ourselves paralyzed into a negation of all criticism. I well remember how afraid we were of our psychologists. It should be noted that this passage was written in the present tense, passive voice, and third person. “The shaman who is believed to be. . . .” There have been changes in psychological anthropology. Unlike Parker, what Vincent Crapanzano did in Tuhami (1980) was to lend himself into the

psychological process. He included himself in it, and as a result he gave our discipline a strong sympathy for his rather disturbed friend (not much more disturbed than many of us). Crapanzano was among the pioneers of the “I” in anthropology, and helped to give anthropology its new honesty. What Edie Turner is doing at this moment is suspecting that Tuhami actually did have an afreet, a kind of demon or succubus, not quite a witch, a darker version of which Stoller experienced in Niger, and others have admitted to having experienced also. Also, in the case of Weyer’s “Eski- mos,” is it out of the question that the shaman was indeed battling against some evil thing for the good of his people? One thing is sure: Weyer him- self never had that experience.

Let us look at some examples of the cold kind of anthropology. Looking through the early British literature on Africa, I strangely cannot find any- thing that is really cold, although I used to think it was all cold. The warmth of the wood fires and the mud huts is in it. We have to go to the cruelty of Lévi-Strauss:

The ultimate goal of the human sciences is not to constitute man, but to dissolve him. The critical importance of ethnology is that it represents the first step in a pro- cess that includes others. Ethnological analysis tries to arrive at invariants beyond the empirical diversity of societies [when using “empirical diversity” here, Levi- Strauss is meaning the opposite to generalizations—he appears to be unaware of William James’s (1976) idea of radical empiricism. Later radical empiricists, Michael Jackson (1989), and others, have made significant discoveries, drawing certain generalizations from a different class of diversities]. . . . This initial enter- prise opens the way for others . . . which are incumbent on the natural sciences: the reintegration of culture into nature and generally of life into the whole of its physicochemical conditions. (quoted in Geertz 1993:346)

This is an attractive project, but deadly, as most anthropologists would understand now. It was a great mistake for Lévi-Strauss to say this—a brave attempt, but it landed him in an untenable position. Anyone can see that this “enterprise” is signally not our business and that it is far from incumbent on us. It is the rationalist’s cause-and-effect dogma, invented by the culture item called Western science. Alan Campbell, one of the most genuine and thoughtful of the new anthropologists, says (1989:169), “The vocabulary of structuralism . . . was a disastrous murrain that laid the dis- cipline low during the 1960s with still prominent results.”

Even Western science’s chaos theory has proved that. The paths and steps of causality, and even the most rational and mechanical roots of social action, are jogged into irregularity at every point. Along the way of rationalism are mists in the nature of things.

Then we have the kind of survey funded by the Lilly Foundation, to count the number of Christian Small Communities (CSC) in America, and to statistically assess their “spirituality.” This is within the discipline of

sociology, with an anthropological advisor—myself—of whom the statis- ticians took no notice. The group is now beginning to publish. What will they see? A group of numbers. They are using the wrong tools. I actually joined a Christian small group and have experienced the most compli- cated, beautiful, and effective initiation process I have ever encountered. I am still trying to figure out what happened. But I am the guinea pig. Sta- tistics of whatever sort would be almost useless here.

What about hesitations? This is a rather sensitive topic, but we have to face it. Alan Campbell (1989:90), speaking of the spirit entities of the Wayapi Indians of the Amazon, and in the process of siding passionately with the Indians in their views, says nevertheless, “What deserves empha- sis is not just that the [magical] creatures are said to be there, but that these images are metaphors through which the living world is expressed.” That is, when all is said and done, there are no magical creatures. The idea in the passage is they must have been invented to supply an image of the society to itself.

Even so, after being so ambiguous, so reductionist, Campbell goes on to say, “The [creatures] are not just extra entities. And they are certainly more than our casual figure of personification. They are the vehicle by which the living tenor is conceived” (90). Now this merges into what I myself want to say about the importance of such entities. “The vehicle”: we have popped through the curtain here, through Alice’s mirror. Those entities are the vehicle of whole peoples’ whole lives. We have to know more about them for a start. We have to feel what they are. It so happened that poor Campbell mistakenly shot one of the Wayapi’s most sacred beings, an ana- conda. This was indeed a fault in involvement, showing graphically how much he hesitated to recognize the sacredness of those beings.

Again, some, like Michael Bourdillon (1997) researching religion in Harare, admit that no, they never had a religious experience among their field people. There seems to be something stopping such people. Students sometimes confess (as if it were a shortcoming and will get them low grades) that they do not “get” shamanism. In other sectors there begins to be a complaint at the elitist pride of the experiencers, those who do get it. Is there something in their complaint? It frightens me to think that such people will feel they are out of it, because that fact makes me want to give up altogether trying to experience these things myself. How can I be so hostile as to distance myself from those whose only fault is taking the rationality of anthropology seriously? My argument has been that all I wanted to be was their guinea pig and try the experiences on myself, much as my son Bob, in his brain research lab in London, often gets into the magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) giant magnet himself so that the experiments can go on.

This problem of experience now appears to be a divisive one in our dis- cipline. Nevertheless the experiencing types are so peripheral to anthro-

pology that the nonexperiencers should have no sleepless nights over the matter. In fact, the two types badly need each other for balance and—shall we say it—sanity. The overrational cannot be sane without sympathy for the religions of others, and the religious could go overboard.

About the experiencers, I would maintain, we do have to treat their material with respect. Paul Stoller (1984:110) was bewitched, and this was his final induction into the heart of Songhai knowledge, told in straight- forward narrative style, just as in the case of Bruce Grindal’s (1983) wit- ness of a dead man dancing among the Sisala in 1967. These occurrences were not just trivial matters but some of the big events of anthropology. Why? Because these researchers were taking long steps over into a field that was closed in anthropology, closed through the same prejudice that makes researchers afraid of their own emotions.

Going back into the thorny matter of the emotions, what kind of emo- tion has ruled among anthropologists working with such so-called magic? A large number have satisfied themselves that there was some cheating involved—and these have therefore redirected their interest to the impli- cations and effects of the ritual in the social or political sphere. Others, who did experience an unusual phenomenon where no cheating could have been involved, such as Evans-Pritchard’s sighting of a witch light among the Azande (1976:11), have briefly recorded the event and contin- ued on as if nothing had happened; and from the conversations in kitchens and corridors back home, it appears that the individual con- cerned was pleasantly puzzled, safe in the knowledge that anthropology did not require “an explanation” (Peter Huber, Roy Wagner, personal communication, although the latter is forging ahead with a new frame- work to deal with the phenomena). Others again have been truly startled (rarely, I have found, positively scared), and the events have stimulated them to fight the philosophical battle involved with all their might (Stoller 1984; Favret-Saada 1980; Goulet 1998, to name a few).

But what of the mistakes, the false directions in which this philosophy might go? Anthropology, to its credit, has remained remarkably free of the Lynn Andrews taint of New Age “anthropology,” through which a lot of money has been made. Oddly enough, the master popularizer and collec- tor of customs and symbols, ancient and modern, Joseph Campbell, lies within this category, and Mircea Eliade has been shoved into another side- line by mainline anthropology.

Among those anthropologists who do experience the religion or nonordi- nary powers of their field people, their styles and methods vary a great deal. The vast majority of the publications that approximate to something like the anthropology of experience verge on the side of hedging their bets and show a tendency to rationalize. Their fall is backward, not forward. Exam- ples of overdoing the process, falling forward, occur when someone is extremely interested in nonordinary events and wants to give them the way

they happened, but treats them too solemnly. I myself have fallen forward in that way. Generally, there has been, I feel, an overearnestness in raw tape- recording. For instance, were the sounds Carol Laderman (1991) tape- recorded in Malaysia at Sacred Wind trance ceremonies the sounds of spirits speaking? The transcription went on for 54 odd pages of her book without break. Weighing up the matter we can see that these were indeed the voices of spirits coming through a human medium. But there are no stage direc- tions beyond the barest and most impersonal ones, no running exegesis or thoughts, reactions, or even emotions of the ethnographer, who was present with her tape recorder. There seems to be an assumption that the transcrip- tion of spirit voices is a kind of Bible. We can see this attitude in the post- modernism of Stephen Tyler and in the new school of critical anthropology at the rise of George Marcus and others. Here Tyler is proposing the sacred- ness of the ethnographic text: “The model of postmodern ethnography is not the newspaper but that original ethnography—the Bible” (Tyler 1986:127). So in Laderman’s case we receive little commentary in the long passages in which the sounds are extensively transcribed. Somehow this is Bible. We see somewhat of a failure in taking the experience closely to her- self, and to us the readers—after she had completed describing her own trance, in remarkable and indeed award-winning fashion. I would appeal to anthropologists, let us not suddenly act holy about field material, not sacral- ize it. Such sacralization is seen in one of Tyler’s extreme passages, “The break with everyday reality is a journey apart into strange lands with occult practices—into the heart of darkness—where fragments of the fantastic whirl about in the vortex of the quester’s disoriented consciousness, until, arrived at the maelstrom’s center, he loses consciousness at the very moment of the miraculous, restorative vision, and then, unconscious, is cast up onto the familiar, but forever transformed, shores of the commonplace world” (1986:126). Here there is not even any story as in Laderman’s case, but an attempt to reproduce a kind of superdramatic ritual initiating some quester into the occult world of the postmodern. A bit of genuine personal experience here, in the past tense, active voice, and first person, would have made all the difference. If it were not for Tyler’s magnificent championing— theoretically—of the “I” in ethnography and call for freedom from enforced systematizing, one would think we were with Maya Deren or Malidoma Somé, practitioners and not anthropologists.

So anthropology has its own mission, that of keeping a kind of interna- tional consciousness going—on behalf of the planet that really wants to know what is going on in the rest of itself. Continual commentary is indis- pensable for this. We have to study this “world-Bible” of translogical material, tear into it, see what stuff it is made of, not put it into a glass case as a sacred text. Our comments as international anthropologists, plus the practitioners’ comments in close dialogue, are going to create the style of future anthropology.

What of experience of what is not cultural, not social, experiences that are enabled in a social milieu but are not primarily for society, in no way scientific or economic, but startling and central in the eyes of many soci- eties—I mean healing? If one has been healed by a field friend, what then? Treating the matter in its own right has been strongly avoided by our anthropological community, truly tabooed. Only a mischievous person like myself keeps picking open these closed cases and saying, “You know! They matter.” And there is a dead silence. Is it all too difficult?

I was in the Iñupiat village of Point Hope one April, doing some follow- up fieldwork, when I contracted flu, followed by a shattering headache. It was a sunny late evening and everyone was at Bingo: the healer Dorcus, the healer Rosella, healer Henry, healer Dinah, and everybody else. I wan- dered piteously through the snowbound village, calling on certain doors and drawing a blank. I wanted a healer. The Tylenol was not working.

I did go to the house of old Dinah, 80. Surely she could not be out, but she was. It must have been the $1,000 night at the Bingo hall, a time of tense excitement. Intervening in the proceedings in the hall was unthink- able. So now in Dinah’s living room, jumbled about by grandchildren turning their black eyes cheerfully upon their erstwhile adoptive grandma who used to play origami with them, I got the answer, “She’s out.”

I thought for a moment. “Diva’s not here, is she?” Diva was the well- named daughter of the healer Rosella, and niece of Dinah. This teenage woman had a vigorous mop of black hair, an active sturdy frame, and a daring, adventurous style in her doings with the village. She was 19 and the mother of three children. She was a brilliant basketball player on the