Theses
Thesis/Dissertation Collections
8-1-1994
Color reproduction of CRT-displayed images as
projected transparencies
Audrey A. Lester
Follow this and additional works at:
http://scholarworks.rit.edu/theses
This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Thesis/Dissertation Collections at RIT Scholar Works. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses by an authorized administrator of RIT Scholar Works. For more information, please [email protected].
Recommended Citation
Audrey A. Lester
B.S. State University College at Brockport (1978)
A thesis submitted for partial fulfillment
of the requirements for the degree of
Master of Science
in
Color Science
in
the Center for Imaging Science
in the College of Imaging Arts and Sciences
of the Rochester Institute and Technology
August 1994
Audrey Lester
Signature of Author
CERTIFICATE OF AFPROVAL
MS.
DEGREE THESIS
The
MS.
Degree Thesis of Audrey A. Lester
has been examined and approved
by two members of the color science faculty
as satisfactory for the thesis requirement for the
Master of Science degree.
Dr. Mark D. Fairchild, Thesis Advisor
Dr. Roy S. Berns
Rochester Institute of Technology
Center for Imaging Science
Title of Thesis:
Color Reproduction of CRT Displayed Images as Projected Transparencies
I,
Audrey Lester
Ihereby grant permission to the Wallace Memorial
Library of
RI.T. ,
to reproduce my thesis in whole or in part. Any reproduction
will not be for commercial use or profit.
Date:
--J..J...tt;j~/....(,-q
-I-L/ _ _ I7
jAudrey
A. LesterSubmitted forpartial
fulfillment
oftherequirementsforthedegreeofMasterofSciencein ColorScience intheCenterfor
Imaging
Science inthe CollegeofImaging
ArtsandSciencesoftheRochester Instituteof
Technology
ABSTRACT
This thesis evaluated the color appearance predictions of four digital color
transforms
(Hunt,
RLAB, CIELAB,
and vonKries)
between CRT "original"images,
viewed in a lighted room, and projected slides, viewed in the dark. Calibrated projection of these images required resolution of several complexissues. Upon projection, the slide colors changed. A rapid color shift (on the
order of 75 seconds) was followed
by
a slower,steady
degradation which alsohad to beminimized for accurateimage presentation.
Therefore,
a model ofthe film behavior was based upon dye absorptivities and color measurements ofslides as
they
were projected.The psychophysical experiments included a comparison between
preference choicesandmemory matching to theCRT
"original."
Two CRTwhite
points were evaluated: D93 and D65. The preference choices were, in
fact,
distinct from the selected matches. RLAB producedstatistically
superior matches overany
other model for both white points. Model performance wasimage dependent.
Occasionally,
CIELAB or von Kries images were equivalentto RLAB.
However,
CIELAB and von Kries predictions rangedwidely
in theirperformance. Hunt's image predictions consistently gave the worst results.
Interestingly,
RLAB was also elected as the most acceptable choice: judged 'acceptable'inmorethan2/3ofallcases,witha maximum approval of89%.
contributors. First of all, Mark Fairchild and
Roy
Berns implemented the filmrecorder model. Mark also wrote the C programs to calculate the digital color
transforms. Lisa Reniffprovided suggestions and support for
many metrology
concerns. ColleenDesimoneoversawthe
purchasing
issues.Additionally,
thisproject wassponsoredby
awidediversity
of governmentand industrialfunding:
NSF-NYS/IUCRC
NYSSTF-CAT CenterforElectronic
Imaging
SystemsManagement
Graphics,
Inc.Xerox Corporation
Realization of such asignificant personal achievement could nothave been
possible without the assistance of
family
members, preschool teachers andfriends who pitched in when
unsuspecting
crises arose. Thanks to Ruth andJohn
Klipp,
Joan and NelsonLester,
Betty
Sheridan,
RobertaDiNoto,
Amy
Stappenbeck,
and AnneHoenig,
who made it possible for me to reenter thetechnical arena.
Most
importantly,
special recognitionbelongs
tomy husband
and childrenfortheir continualencouragement,patience andsupport.
ListofTables xiii
1. Introduction 1
1.1. Visual Perceptionand
Cross-Media
ColorReproduction 11.2. OrganizationofThesis 1
2. Phase I-- CalibratedProjection
of35mmslides 3
2.1. Background 3
2.1.1. Film
Stability
andDegradation 32.1.2. SpectralChangesUpon Projection 5
2.1.3. Film Recorder Model 6
2.1.3.1. Beer-BouguerLaw 8
2.1.3.2.
Estimating
DyeConcentrations 82.1.3.3. Dye Concentrations as PredictorsofDigitalCounts 11
2.2. The EffectofProjection 14
2.2.1. Equipment 15
2.2.2. Experiment 1:
Resolving
SpectralDifferences 162.2.2.1. Initial
Study
162.2.2.2. Cool Down
Study
172.2.2.3. Results 17
2.2.2.3.1. Regression Correction 17
2.2.2.3.2. Fluorescence 21
2.2.2.3.3.
Cool-down
vs. spectral 222.2.3. Experiment 2: Evaluation Of Color Shifts 27
2.2.3.1.
Stability
ofthe ColorChange
272.2.3.2. Color
Change
With Repeated5 MinuteProjection 272.2.3.3. Results 28
2.2.3.3.1.
Chromaticity
Change Over Time 282.2.3.3.2. Repeated Projection 28
2.3. Film RecorderCalibration 35
2.3.1. Experiment 3: Characteristic Vectors 35
2.3.2. Experiment4: Tristimulus
Matching
372.3.2.1. SlideMeasurement 37
2.3.2.2.
Modeling
Design 382.3.2.3. Results: EvaluationofModel
Accuracy
392.3.3. Experiment5: Image
Variability
392.3.3.1.
Gray Variability
402.3.3.2. Precision
Study
432.3.3.3.
Processing
Variability
442.3.3.4. Slide Degradation DuetoProjection 46
2.3.4. Film RecorderSettings 49
2.3.5. Recommendations 53
3. Phase II--CRTtoSlideAppearance Model
Testing
553.1. Background 55
3.1.1. Visual System 55
3.1.1.1. Color
Matching
Functions andVisualPrimaries 563.1.1.2. Rod Intrusion 57
3.1.1.3. Background Effects 58
3.1.1.3.1. Helson-Judd Effect 59
3.1.1.4. Surround Effects 59
3.1.1.5. Cognitive Effects 61
3.1.1.6. Chromatic Adaptation 62
3.1.1.7. Incomplete Adaptation 63
3.1.2. Color ReproductionCalculations 64
3.1.2.1. vonKries 64
3.1.2.2. CIELAB 65
3.1.2.3. RLAB 67
3.1.2.4. Hunt 69
3.1.3. Field TrialsofColorAppearanceTransforms 72
3.1.4. Experimental Techniques 74
3. 1.4.1. Direct Comparisonof
Viewing
Techniques 743.1.5. Statistical Evaluation
Techniques/Theory
763.2. Experimental 79
3.2.1.
Facility
Design andSet-up
793.2.2. Image SelectionandPreparation 82
3.2.2.1. Gamut Issues 83
3.2.3.
Modeling
Parameters 843.2.3.1. CalculationofGraysforImageBackground 85
3.2.3.2. CRT White Points 86
3.2.4. Experimental Procedure 87
3.3. Results andDiscussion 88
3.3.1 Preferencevs.
Matching
Choices
893.3.2 EvaluationofModelMatches 89
3.3.2.1.
Repeatability
963.3.3.
Acceptability
964.1 Conclusion 100
References 102
Appendix 1: RegressionAnalysis for Inter-InstrumentSpectral Correction 107
Appendix 2: SpectraoftheLeicaandKodakCarouselProjectorSources 110
Appendix 3: Colorimetric Data forFilm Recorder Calibration 112
Appendix 4: Tone ReproductionTables forFilm Recorder Model 114
Appendix 5: CRT Calibration Model 121
Model
Accuracy
123Appendix 6: PreCalc Files With Intermediate Variable ValuesforHunt '93 and
Fairchild '91 Color Appearance Models 125
Appendix 7: Observer Instructions 127
Appendix 8: Observer Profiles andRaw Psychophysical Results 129
D93 130
D65 138
Appendix 9: CalculationsofZ-ScoresandConfidence Intervals 146
Experiment 1: All
Images, Preference,
D93 WhitePoint 146Experiment 2: All
Images,
ImageMatching,
D93WhitePoint 150Experiment 3: All
Images,
Preference,
D65 WhitePoint 154FIG. 3. Spectraltransmittance for single-colorEktachrome slides 19
FIG. 4. Thermochromicshiftfor thegreenslide 23
FIG. 5.
AL*, ACab*,
andAHab*betweenhot
andcoolslides 25FIG. 6.
(a*,b*),
L*,
andCab* changeswhile yellowslidecools 26FIG. 7.
Chromaticity
coordinatesof3slideswhile projectedover time 29FIG. 8. Repeatedprojectionofthe magenta slide 30
FIG. 9. Color shiftbetweenprojected androomtemperature slides 31
FIG. 10. Colorchange after20minutes of projection 32
FIG. 11. 'Projected' dye absorptivities 36
FIG. 12.
Gray
slidevariabilityas afunctionofexposure/measurementday....41FIG. 13.
Gray
slidevariabilility as afunctionofsurrounding
colors 42FIG. 14. Colorerror compared with positionontheroll offilm 44
FIG. 15.
Variability
ofcontrol strips 45FIG. 16. Slide degradationduetoprojection 48
FIG. 17.
Density
ofgraysfromvarious CalSets(LineScans),
andLUTs 52FIG. 18. Dimensions andspacingoftheexperimental area 80
FIG. 19. Image dimensions
during
matching
and preferenceexperiments 81FIG. 20. Imagesusedinpsychophysicalexperiments 82
FIG. 21. Experimental viewingconditions 88
FIG. 22. Preference choices vs. matchestoCRT
'original'
90
FIG. 23. D93
matching
experiment: z-scores andconfidence intervals 91FIG. 24. D65
matching
experiment: z-scores and confidenceintervals 92FIG. 25. D93 preference experiment: z-scoresand confidence intervals 94
FIG. 26. D65preference experiment: z-scoresand confidenceintervals 95
FIG. 27. Spectraofthe Leicaand Kodakprojectorsources Ill
TableIII. Comparisonoftwofilmrecorder models 39
TableIV. Color degradation
due
toprojection(numberofobservers) 49Table V. Influenceoffilmrecorder settings on
gray
exposure 53TableVI. Average AEab*
caused
by
exposure,processing
or projection 54TableVII. Measuredmodelparameters 85
Table VHI.
Acceptability
as afractionofimagematching
97 Table IX.Summary
ofimageacceptability
ranges 98Table X. EvaluationofCRTmodel,bothwhite points 124
1.1. Visual Perception and
Cross-Media
Color ReproductionImagesreproduced
colorimetrically may
notappearthe same when viewedacross differentmedia. Visual colorperceptionis acomplex combination oflight
stimulation, signal processing, and cognitive interpretation.
Thus,
perceptioncan notbe
simply described
by
the reflectance of an object. A quantification ofthe brain's final interpretation of color must be determined in order to
successfully
predict the appearance of an image when it is presented underdifferent viewing conditions.
Gauging
this interpretation has proven tobe complex. Documented colorappearance phenomena illustrate the unusual behavior of our perception.
Several effects,
directly
related to this work, will be reviewed. In the past,because of these complexities, color identification and color difference formulas
were devised after stringently controlling viewing conditions
(Bartleson,
1981).Today,
with the technologicalavailability
for novice users to create imagesacross media, a growing demand to produce colors that appear the same on
output as those created on a
CRT,
for example, has sparked current research toevaluate color appearance models that predict color matches across different
media.
1.2. Organization ofThesis
Without accurate modeling of the film recorder and careful attention to all
experiments are dubious.
Many
details
of this worklogically
fall into twodistinct
categories: thoseinvolving
creation of calibrated slide images andpsychophysical
testing
of color reproduction formulas.Therefore,
this thesisconsists of two sections. Within the
first
sectionmany
calibration issues are discussed. This work has quantified the effect of heat from the projector lightsource,
variability
of single-color-slide colorsdue
toprocessing
and the filmrecorder,
modeling
error from the CRT and film recorder, and degradation ofthe colors that mightbe expected throughouta psychophysical experiment with
15 observers. Some other effects have been
empirically
identified as concerns,such as changes intransmittance for patches made with the same digital counts but surrounded
by
different colors. Several settings for the film recorder werealsoevaluated.
The section on psychophysics develops several topics: specific factors that
affect the visual system's interpretation of the input stimuli, experimental
techniques, previous
testing
of color appearance models, color reproduction models (inthisexperiment) andevaluation of results.Many
theories have been developed toquantify
the visual perception ofcolor. This thesis used a
memory
matching
technique to evaluate aforced-choice, paired-comparison psychophysical experiment
using
color reproduction formulae from four models: Hunt93, RLAB, CIELAB,
and von Kries. These2.1. Background
2.1.1.
Film
Stability
andDegradation
Photographic film is comprised of an
extremely
complex system ofchemicals
containing
multiple functional groups.Many
possible types of undesirable chemical interactions causedby
avariety offactors can occurin thissystem. The filmusedinthis experimentwas Ektachrome 100 Plus
Professional,
a chromogenic
(dye-coupling)
photographic system in which dyes are formedfrom colorless couplers
already
incorporated into the film. Two types of colorchanges have been well-documented in chromogenic systems:
bleaching
and staining (Wilhelm andBrower,
1993).Bleaching
(orfading)
results whenever thedensity
ofdye isreduced incomparisontotheoriginalimage,
whilestaining
refers to the formation of unwanted yellow product. An explanation forbleaching
begins with an understanding of what causes color in these compounds. At the heart of all chemical processes is the requirement forelectrons to reside in stable electronic states. These states account for the
presence of absorption bands as well as potential chemical reactivity. For
example, a molecule can absorb a quantumonly ifthe
energy
of the quantum isthe same as the energy difference between the current electronic state and a
higher state ofthemolecule. Some compounds with a series of
alternating
singleand double bonds (called conjugated
bonds)
are known to haveenergy
capable of
absorbing
light with wavelengthsranging
from 400 to 700 nm, thuscausing
the appearanceof color.Wheneverchanges inthe electronic states occur, theabsorption peaks change
as well. Forexample,stereochemical forms oftenhave differentpeak absorption
wavelengths since the cis- isomer is
more constrained
(requiring
moreenergy
tovibrate). If the conjugated
double-bond
structure of the chromophore isshortened, greater
energy
is needed to push the electrons into the next stablelevel. This means that shorter wavelengths (instead of visible ones) will be
absorbed
(Zollinger,
1991). The dyewillbecome colorless. Ineffect, the amountof absorptionatthe original wavelengthisreduced.
Many
acidic types ofreagentswill add to the double bonds and degrade thesequence ofconjugatedbonds. Even
humidity
(orwater) plays a vital roleinthefading
rates of dyes throughhydrolysis or hydroxylation. Water is also knownto facilitate the generation of hydrogen peroxide (that has been shown to be
present
during
many photooxidations)(Zollinger,
1991). Illuminationmay
alsoinfluence the degradation. The energy derived from the absorption of
ultraviolet radiation can produce the formation of excited states in the
bonds;
theseare thenableto react with atmospheric oxygentoreducethe double bond.
Oxidation of the dyes or other chemicals inthe photographic emulsion,
may
form stains that are yellow or brown.
Quinones,
for example are yellow.Notably,
naphthoquinones have been formed from the degradation of azo dyeswith peroxide
(Nassau,
1985). (Color couplersfrequently
contain an azo-typelinkage.)
Heatwill accelerate the rate ofreactionthereby
causing, over a certainInsummary,
many
causes are knownto affectthe rate of film degradation:chemical
stability
of the color photographic materials, storage conditions (temperature andhumidity),
display
conditions(intensity
ofillumination,
duration of exposure, spectral distribution of the
illuminant,
ambientenvironmental conditions
(temperature
andhumidity)
andquality
of thechemical
processing
(incompletewashing
orremoval of photographic products
-including
silver,halide
ions,
ordevelopers).2.1.2. Spectral Changes
Upon ProjectionKnowing
projection of slides causesdegradation,
initial measurements ofsingle-color slides (for characterization of the film recorder) were made on a
spectrophotometer
(using
both regular and total geometries). The slides were then projected and measured with a spectroradiometer. The spectral curveswere compared to determine which
geometry
could be used to estimate the projected spectral transmittance.However,
spectral incongruities between theprojected andunprojected slideswere nonuniform and varied for differentslide
colors. These differenceswere later showntobe caused
by
projection. Themostlikely
cause would be thermochromism due toheat
from the projectorlight
source. Other possible explanations for the spectral changes could be
photochromismorhydrochromism (lossof water).
A thermochromic shift differs from dye degradation and stain formation
because it is reversible. This suggests a mechanism in which a shift in
equilibrium (catalyzed
by
heat)
causes aminor change inthe chromophore, thusliterature
(Zollinger,
1991)
and evaluated in standard reference materials (Grumand
Fairchild, 1985);
however,
no published literature on thermochromism ofphotographic film could be found. This phenomenon will be discussed in
greaterdetaillater(Lester and
Fairchild,
1994).2.1.3.
Film Recorder Model
As stated
by
Berns(1993),
processed filmmay
be considered as a firstapproximation, a transparent medium
consisting
of dyed gelatin. Given thisassumption, the
relationship
between digital input to the film recorder andresulting film transmittance was predicted using:
1)
the Beer-Bouger Law(which relates transmittance to concentration),
2)
a modification of thetristimulus matching algorithm described
by
Allen(1984)
and implementedby
Berns
(1993a)
to determinethe concentrations ofdye on thefilm,
3)
a non-linearstage (to model the film's tone-reproduction curve), and
4)
anempirically
derivedmatrixtoacountforinter-imageeffects. Thisis shownin Fig. 1.below.
First of all, the image is
digitally
representedby
three planes(red,
green,and blue). The digital counts are modified non-linearly
by
the film recorder.Interimage effects (or cross-exposure within film layers to improve image
fidelity)
influence the actual dye formation. From dye concentrations andabsorptivities, the Beer-Bouguer Law is used to
spectrally
reconstruct thetransmittance of the image color. Tristimulus values are then calculated from
the transmittance and the spectra of the projector light source. These steps
define the relationship between digital counts and output color (expressed as
3digitalplanes: red,green,blue
Red DigitalCounts
m
c 1_ y_l
(actual)
E GreenDigitalCounts
inter-image matrix
(rowsnormalized
andsumto1)
BlueDigitalCounts
c
l_ y_l
(predicted)
SpectralReconstruction fromconcentrationsusing
the Beer-BouguerLaw
u
s
'1
Si
QJ u
fa
E to
Wavelength
SpectraofProjector Source
ReflectedoffScreen
x^
H
Wavelength
X=kZ(S(X)*Tw*T(X))
Y=kI (S{X)*
T{X)
*J{X))Z=kZ(S(X)*Tw*-z(X))
Consequently,
to accurately reproduce colorsthroughexposureby
thefilmrecorder, the
derived
model is inverted so that tristimulus values are used asinput and the required digital counts are calculated. An iterative searching algorithm was developed
by
Mark D. Fairchildto accomplishthis task.2.1.3.1. Beer-Bouguer Law
Given the assumptionthat processedfilm
may
beconsidered a transparentmedium
consisting
of dyed gelatin and that the dyes do not scatter light orfluoresce,
the amount of dye in the film canbe describedby
a simplification ofthe Beer-BouguerLaw:
^measured
=T\gO
*e<*PX,c
+^rrPXfm
+cyDX/y)
(1)
where
T^
measured approximatesT^mternai
(the internaltransmittance),
T^q
is the transmittance of the substate (in this caseDmin,
a slide made from themaximum digital counts), c is the concentration, and
D^
is the spectraldensity
(or absorptivity
)
ofthe cyan,magenta,and yellowdyes.Inordertousethis strategy,it is
necessary
toknow theabsorptivity
of eachdye. While these curves are available from the manufacturer for cool
measurements, anothermethod was needed toderive
absorptivity
for'hot'slides
(since it will be shown that projected slides have unique dye absorptivities).
This was accomplished using principal component analysis from measurements
ofthree sets of
ramp
dataandwillbe discussedinsection2.3.1.2.1.3.2.
Estimating
Dye Concentrationsparts:
determining
the initial matrices,converting
concentration totransmittance,
calculating
tristimulus values, anditeratively
refining
theestimate ofconcentration untilatristimulusmatchisachieved.
Firstofall, the
following
matrices are defined:Tisthematrixofcolor
matching functions
(a3x31matrix):T= ^400 ^410 ^400 ^410 *700 ^700 2700
(2)
Eisthespectral power distributionofthelightsource (a31x31matrix):
E= "400 0 0 0 "410 0 0 0 ^700
(3)
Disa31x31 diagonalmatrix with
d^
=-2.3026 *
T^measured:
~dm
0 00 D=
*400
0
d.
4100 0 d
700.
(4)
<E> represents the dye absorptivities as a function of wavelength (a 31x3
matrix):
0=
*
(yellow)400 ^(magenta)0
400 Y400xXcyan)xAyeUow) xAmagenta) Jpicyan)
^410 Y410 Y410
xAyellow) ^(magenta) jAcyan) S,700 Y700 Y700
cis thenormalized concentration matrix (3x1):
^yellow..
"
magenta
(6)
/
is the absorption spectrum for the color of interest (a 31x1 matrix). K isthe absorptioncoefficient expressed as the-log(T^measured
/
T^go)-A
400410
K700
(7)
Concentrations arefirstestimated
by
thefollowing:c = (TEDO)-!TEDf
(8)
Fromtheestimated concentrations andthespectral dye
absorptivity
curves,the transmittance can be derived from the Beer-Bouguer equation. The
tristimulus values are calculated from the transmittance curves.
Next,
thedifferencebetweenthe predicted and actualtristimulus values
(AX, AY,
andAZ)
areusedtoobtainacorrectionmatrix,usingthe
following
formula:Ac =
(TEDO)"1
At
(9)
where,
At=
AX
AY
AZ
(10)
and, Ac =
Ac
Ac
Ac
yellow..
magenta
(11)
Acyellow
/Acmagenta
, andAcCyan
refer to an iterative correction in theconcentrations. Anew concentration vectoriscalculatedfromtheexpression:
Cnew
=C0ld + (Ac*0.4)
(12)
The algorithm iterates until an
acceptably
small At is derived. A multiplicative factor of 0.4 was used to assure convergence. At that point, a reasonableestimateforthe concentrationhas been found.
Upon
implementation,
spectral data was difficult to obtain with sufficientprecision
necessitating
colorimetric measurements using a tristimulus filter colorimeter. In this case, the D matrix was calculatedby
setting
T^
measured to0.4and thefirstestimate ofdye concentrations was givenby:
ccyan
= 1_X/XnCmagenta= 1"Y/Yn
(13)
cyellow= *" Z/Zn
2.1.3.3. Dye Concentrationsas
Predictors
ofDigitalCounts
The next step is to determine the
relationship
between the dyeconcentration and the known digital counts of the input images. After
estimating dye concentrations
(using
the tristimulusmatching
algorithm described above), three tone reproduction curves were derived forgray
colors(those
having
equal digital counts). A cubic spline interpolation method wasLevels
FIG. 2. Tonereproductioncurves
relating
digital countstodye concentration.If the
dyes
in color film were independent upon exposure, the tonereproduction curves would be sufficient to describe the
relationship between
digital count and
dye
concentration. However asdescribed
by
Berns (1993):"...color film relies on the interactionbetween
dyes
toimprove
colorreproduction fidelity. This well-known
inter-image
effect must be taken into account. This was accomplishedby
exposing
film with a factorial designwhere red, green, and blue ramps wereindividually
variedwhile theothertwochannelswereset at amediumInterimage effects canbe seeninthedata throughexamination ofpredicted
dye concentrationwiththeincrease in digital countsofoneinterimage ramp at a
time. If the dyes were
independent,
the concentration of the other dyes wouldremain constant.
However,
as the exposure of animage becomes more red, forexample,not
only
doesthe cyan concentrationdramatically drop
off,butalso theyellow and magenta concentrations increaseslightly.
If each of these changes were
linear,
a 3x3 transformation consisting ofslopes would model the interaction. The diagonal elements would contain
relatively
large positive numbers while the off-diagonals would have smallnegative values. The actual data are not so well behaved. In one study, this
obstacle was overcome
by
replacing
the simple slopes of the hypothetical 3x3matrix with a matrix of spline functions
(Berns,
1993). However in this thesis,error in the data points from processing and measurement variations produced
erratic spline functions.
Modeling
error wasparticularly
obvious for thegray
colors. Inthe end,a3x3matrix gavethebestresults.
The matrix was derived in the
following
manner.Using
18 new slidecolors, actual dye concentrations (calculated
by
the tristimulusmatching
algorithm) and predicted dye concentrations (derived through the tone
reproduction curves) were determined. As
previously
implied,
ifthere were nointer-image effects, these two concentrations would be equal (within
experimental noise). A linear regression model was calculated for each dye
concentration. The model was constrained so that each row summed to one. This restriction was used to maintain
gray
balance. Theresulting
inter-image"yellow...
''magenta
1.0542 -.0455
-.1332 1.0125 0.1207
0.0040 -.0081 1.0040
"yellow..
"magenta
cyan...
(14)
The adjusted concentrations were used to reconstruct the spectral
transmittance ofthe predicted slide color
using
the Beer-Bouguer Law.Finally,
tristimulus values were calculated from the spectraltransmittance,
projectorsource and color
matching
functions.Applying
the inverse of this model, tristimulus values are transformed totransmittance then to dye concentrations. The inter-image matrix is inverted to
changespectrally derivedconcentrations to themodified concentrationsused for
the tone reproduction
look-up
tables.Finally,
an iterative searching algorithmrelates this second set of concentrations to digital counts. These input values
should result in the correct exposure
by
the Solitaire8xp
film recorder, onEktachrome 100 Plus Professionalfilm.
2.2. The Effect of Projection
Aswas statedpreviously,projectionis knowntocause degradationof slide
film. Thus initial work attempted to find a measurement method which would
not degrade the
film,
yet couldbe correlated withthe spectral data of projectedcolors. The colorimetric accuracy ofthese images wouldbe critical for Phase II:
psychophysical tests.
First,
single-color slides were created on a Solitaire filmrecorder, processed, and measured on a spectrophotometer
(using
both regularand total geometries). These slides were then projected and measured with a
2.2.1. Equipment
Avarietyofinstruments wereused tomeasurethe film. Inorder to reduce
degradation from projection, a BYK Gardner Color Sphere
(BKYCS)
spectrophotometer was used to measure the transmittance of the ambient
temperature
("cool")
slides. Thisdouble beam instrument is designedwitha d/8geometry, a quartzhalogen
lamp
withanIRfilter,
a 10 nmbandpass,
a concaveholographic
grating
spectrograph and a solid state silicon diodearray
detector.Both regular and total transmittance can be measured on samples with a
diameter at leastthe width of35 mm slides. The slides were measured atroom
temperaturesincethe opticaldesignoftheBYKCSdoes notsignificantly heatthe
object
being
measured.Later,
measurements were made while the slide was projected. A PhotoResearch PR703A spectroradiometer
(PR703A)
was used to record thetransmittance of projected slides at 0 from the normal to the projection screen.
This is a single-beam instrument with a 5 nm
bandpass,
a holographicdiffraction
grating
anda256elementphotodiodearray.An LMT-C1200 Colormeter
(LMT)
was used to measure tristimulus valuesof the single-color, 2-color and 9-color projected slide test targets used for
modeling (this will be discussed further). The LMT was also used to
quickly
track chromaticity changes in the projected colors. The projected light was
imaged
directly
onto the detector.Finally,
the LMT was used to measure theCRT channel chromaticities (needed for Phase II). "This instrument is a
tristimulus colorimeter with nearly perfect filter correction. [It consists of four
tristimulusvalue; onefor the blueregion and one for thered region.] Itis more
accurate and precise than available spectroradiometers due to calibration and
signal-level difficulties."
(Fairchild,
1990,
AppendixC).The projector screen, a Clarion Wall Mounted model
rigidly
stretched on ablack aluminumframe with a vinyl matte white surface
(M1300),
was chosen toprovidethemost
uniformly diffuse
reflectance over abroad
viewingangle.A LeicaP-2200 projector witha
flat
field lenswas used. The temperatureofa projected
gray
slide (witha visualdensity
of2.14)
was 52C measured with aYSI Series 400 thermistor using a Fluke 73 multimeter,
following
the guidelinesfrom ANSI Z38.7.5-1948
(Woodlief,
1973). (This temperature is far below themaximum recommendedtemperatureof
70C.)
2.2.2. Experiment 1:
Resolving
Spectral Differences
2.2.2.1. Initial
Study
Asetof 24 slides were createdwith a Solitaire-16 film recorder. Thecolors
were chosen to provide a somewhat uniform
sampling
throughout the CIELABcolor space. At
first,
the slides were unmounted anddirectly
measured in aBYKCS spectrophotometer using both regular and total transmittance
geometries. Nextthe slides were placed in glassmounts,projected onto a white
matte screen and measured with a PR703A spectroradiometer. This instrument
measures spectral radiance of the projected slide. Spectral transmittance values
ofthe filmwere calculated
by
dividing
the spectralradiance of each slideby
thespectral radiance of a blank slide (in the same orientation as the other
2.2.2.2. Cool Down
Study
A subset of the slides was chosen: dark gray, light gray, cyan, magenta,
yellow, red, green, andblue. These slides were
heated
inthe Leica projector for5minutes thenmeasured inthe BYKCS (regular
transmittance)
asthey
cooled at0, 15, 30, 45, 60, 120, 180, 240, 360,
and 480 seconds. Thespectral datawere usedto calculate CIELAB metrics (for CIE Illuminant A and the 1931 Standard
Colorimetric
Observer).2.2.2.3.
Results
2.2.2.3.1. Regression Correction
Measurements under dissimilar conditions required resolution of two
separate issues: which geometry, regular or total
transmittance,
would beequivalent to the projected
transmittance;
and how differences betweenmeasuring
devices could be resolved in order to compare the spectral data. Atfirst,
it was thought one spectrophotometricgeometry
would show a consistentcorrelationwiththeprojected spectral transmittance measurements.
However,
it becamereadily
apparent that the transmittance curves mismatched in a nonuniformfashionthatvaried for differentslide colors. Figure 3
demonstrates
thisdilemma for three colors. The projected transmittances were used as the
reference spectra for
AEab
calculations. Regular and total geometries weremeasured with the BYKCS using unmounted film at room temperature. As
shown in Table
I,
neither geometry correlated well with the projectedtransmittancecurves. Forthesixmost
highly
chromaticslides, the averageAEab*
geometries. The geometry that produced the lowest
AEab*
varied from slide-to
slide. Most
importantly,
the amount of mismatch was colordependent.Table I. Measurement of spectral
differences
(in terms of AEa^*) betweenBYKCSgeometriesusingthePR703Aspectra asthestandard.
*
indicatesthe
geometry
withthe lowervisualdifferenceSlideColor RegularTransmittance Total Transmittance
Cyan *
4.32 4.58
Magenta * 4.02 5.72
Yellow 4.64 *4.34
Blue * 5.71 7.46
Green *
10.58 10.62
Red 5.13 *4.93
Overall Mean 5.73 6.28
Std Deviation 2.45 2.41
One potential cause for the spectral incongruities would be differences in
instrumental design (for example bandpass). A simple way to simulate the
larger bandpass of the BYKCS when
measuring
with thePR703A,
was toappropriately average the spectral data from the PR703A measurements (at
every
2 nm), resulting in data points with a 10 nm bandpass for both(al
PR703A
-Projected
-+-
BYKCS
-Regular
....o--
BYKCS
-Total
400
500
600
Wavelength
0.3-p a.
0.2-
/I
>^./
0.1-^S.&[
^^^_
^S#?0^ "
1 ' ' ' ' ' ' I *-*
700
After this revision, a regression method to correct for other systematic
spectral differences was examined
(Reniff,
1994;
Robertson, 1987;
and Bernsand
Petersen,
1988). This regression procedure uses a mechanistic model tospectrally
represent the errors as a function of reflectance (ortransmittance)
factor. If a particular parameter is
statistically
significant, the correspondingmeasurement difference between the standard and test values can be
mathematically
corrected to improve correlationbetweenthe spectral data. Thefull model includes the transmittance
(T)
(normalized
between 0 and1), T^, T^,
T^, T^,
and the firstand second derivatives. Thesevariables have a relationshiptodifferenttypesofinstrumental errors: theintercept indicatesthe presence of a
photometriczero error (caused
by
stray
light,
calibrationerror, ormaladjustmentof instrumental optics). Measured transmittance and polynomial terms of the
transmittance indicatephotometric scale error (caused
by
calibration error). Thefirst derivative of the transmittance indicates linear wavelength scale error
(caused
by
a displacement of the diffraction grating, resulting in errorsproportionalto theslope ofthespectraltransmittance). Thesecond derivative of
the transmittance indicates a bandwidth error (caused
by
a variation inconstructionofthe slits surroundingthe monochromator).
Inthe current experiment, a set of five filters: yellow, green,
blue,
neutral,and didymium were measured (Hemmendinger Color
Laboratory
Filter Set:HCL-68). The BYKCS data were considered thereference. The
filters
were thenbacklitwith atungsten-halogen source and measured at
0
from normalwiththe
thermochromicspectral change. The raw PR703A dataweredivided
by
the data forthe sourcebefore theregressioncalculations weremade.Regression analysis was evaluated
using
stepwise,forward,
andbackwardregression. Theregressionwas rerun
using all the variables suggested fromthe
forward
method. This time the partial student's t-test was evaluated to check
significance ofthe variables. Theinterceptwas notsignificant,so the regression
was repeated for the final time to give a R2 of 25%. A residual analysis of the model showed no trends and a normal distribution of the error. (Statistical
outputfromthefinalregressionis included inAppendix
1.)
A small correction for linear wavelength scale (the first
derivative),
bandwidth (the second
derivative)
and photometric error (T2 andT5)
werestatistically required.
However,
theactual changein the spectral data produced an averageAEab*
of
only
0.32 (with a range of 0.08 to 0.46). The spectralvariations werenot explained
by
instrumental differences.2.2.2.3.2. Fluorescence
Although unlikely in slide
film,
anotherpossibility
for the spectraldifference couldhave beenfluorescence. Fluorescentdyes absorb visible
light
atone wavelength and emit visible light at another wavelength. If
fluorescence
were present, differences in the light source
illuminating
the slides could produce inconsistentchanges inthe spectral curves. Asimple comparisonin thespectral data when the slides were illuminated
by
monochromatic orpolychromatic lightwould indicate ifthiswere aconcern.
(Grum,
1980). SeveralMatchScan II spectrophotometer with both monochromatic illumination and
polychromatic illumination. The spectral transmittance from both measurements were identical (within instrumental variability). There was no measurablefluorescence.
2.2.2.3.3. Cool-downvs.spectral
Another
hypothesis
for the color shift was the effect of temperature. Theslides were
noticeably
warm when removed from the projector (while this was not truewhen measurements inthe BYKCSwere made). A quick screen of themedium-gray
slide was runby
warming it in the projector for 5 minutes then using the spectrophotometer toquickly
measure the slide several times as it cooled. The spectral curves changed in the same regions where the discrepancies between the instruments were found. A closer examination ofcyan, magenta, yellow, red, green,
blue,
and two grays was designed with spectra measured at0, 15, 30, 45, 60, 120, 180, 240, 360,
and 480 seconds after projection.The color shiftforthe greenslide is illustrated in Fig. 4. Graph
(a)
depictsthe spectral differences when the slide was measured with the BYKCS at 0 and 480 seconds after
heating
in the projector. The second plot(b),
compares theslide while it was projected,
"hot",
and the "cool" slide (at 480 seconds). The thirdgraph(c),
representsthe spectralchangesbetweenthe "hot"and"cool"
data
for both
(a)
and (b). In this lastdiagram,
the two lines were almost parallel,strongly suggesting the same phenomenon. (The temperature variation
0.3
(a)
BYKCS,
HotBYKCS,
Cool400 500 600 700
Projected, Hot
BYKCS,
Cool
0.05-400 500 600
Wavelength
700'
FIG. 4. Thermochromic color shift for the green slide,
(a)
Hottestmeasurementpossible in the BYKCS compared with the same slide after
cooling
480 seconds,(b)
Measurement (with thePR703A)
while the slide was projected compared withthe same slide measuredwiththe BYKCS aftercooling
480 seconds,(c)
Thedifference
calculation (hotdata
-cool
data)
from both plots(a)
and (b).Notice
thedifferences
exhibit the same patternstrongly
The projected slide exhibited larger transmittance changes than the "hot"
data
measured in the BYKCS.However,
the slides werealready
cooler at 0seconds thanat a steady-state projected temperature.
CIELAB metrics were used to
document
the color changes of the eightslides as
they
cooleddown. As showninFig.5,
shifts inlightness(AL*),
chroma(ACab*),
andhue difference
(AHa^*)
varied unpredictably.Usually
the chromawas greater for the cool slide. Sometimes thelightness decreased upon cooling.
Frequently,
the chroma andhuedifferences
occurred inoppositedirections. TheangleofthechangeinAb*vs. Aa*plots variedforeachcolor. Inotherwords, the
colorsdidnot shiftina singledirection (like yellow)butvariedinhue angle and
chroma inan independentfashion. The change inlightness
(AL*)
and chroma(ACab*)
became asymptotic, with a rapid change for the first 90 seconds whichstarted to level off at about 4 minutes. Figure 6 demonstrates these results for
theyellow slide.
From a calibration viewpoint, the measurements from the
spectrophotometer (at room
temperature)
were undesirable. The actual colorswhich would be viewed upon projection varied in an inconsistent fashion from
FIG. 5. CIELAB changesin lightness
(AL*),
chroma(ACab*),
and hue difference(AHab*)
as each slide cooled down. Noconsistenttrendisobserved.Itshould
be
notedthatheat fromthe projectormay
notbe theonly
possibleexplanation for the observed spectral shifts. Some other factor associated with
projection, such as photochromism or loss of water
(hydrochromism),
thoughunlikely,
may
cause the change in the chromophore. For the purposes of thiswork
however,
it is sufficientto documentthe spectralbehaviorofthefilmwhenprojected. Thus for lack of a better
term,
measurementsduring
projection will(a)
Ab*
-0.1-AL*
-0.25
AC*
0
100
200
300
400
Cooling
Time
(seconds)
100
200
300
400
Cooling
Time
(seconds)
500
FIG. 6. CIELAB changes in
(a*,b*)
coordinates,lightness
(L),
and chroma(Cab*)
2.2.3. Experiment
2:Evaluation Of Color Shifts
2.2.3.1.
Stability
oftheColor Change
Four slides were chosen to
determine
if the color of the slides wouldeventually
stabilize upon projection. Dark and lightgray
were chosen for tonereproduction, and yellow and green were usedsince
they
exhibited the largest AEab*changes in the cool down study. The LMT colorimeter was used to
quickly
measure thechromaticity
coordinates at 15 secondintervalsforthefirst3minutes, thenmeasurements wererecorded at
4, 6, 8, 10, 15,
and20minutes.2.2.3.2.
Color Change With Repeated
5Minute Projection.
Anew setofslideswas preparedfor thisexperiment so that
they
would allhavethe same projectionhistory. This
time,
a subset of8 slides was used (darkgray, medium gray, cyan, magenta, yellow, red, green, and blue). These more
saturated colors exhibitedthelargestcolor shifts.
First,
the slides were mounted and measured with the BYKCS (regulartransmittance).
Next,
each slide was projected for four minutes, aspectroradiometric reading was taken with the PR703A and after a total of five
minutes in the Leica projector, the slide was removed. After
cooling
they
were again measured with the BYKCS. This process was repeated three more times(to give a total projection of
10, 15,
and 20 minutes, respectively). The raw datawere divided
by
data for a blank slide mount beforecalculating
CIELAB2.2.3.3.
Results
2.2.3.3.1.
Chromaticity
Change OverTimeSince the color of the slides underwent a transformation upon projection,
the next question was whether this change would stabilize after some time
period. The LMT colormeter was chosen to
rapidly
monitor the chromaticitycoordinates
(x,
y) ofthe projector output. Figure 7 compares these shifts for theyellow, greenanddark grayslides. Inallthree cases,a rapid color shift(of 75 to
90 seconds) was followed
by
a slower,steady
drift inchromaticity.Apparently
because of projection, two phenomena were
taking
place in which noequilibriumpointwas established.
2.2.3.3.2. Repeated Projection
In Fig.
8,
the magenta slide results were enlarged to illustrate how theCIELAB coordinates alternated between "hot"
and
"cool"
measurements after
5,
10,
15,
and 20 minutes of projection. Figure 9 compares this shift for all sixslides. Upon cooling, the colors almost return back to their original color.
Arrows indicate the general change ofAb*vs. Aa*over time. A drifttoward the
yellow (positive b*
direction)
implies formation of yellow stain causedby
projection.
A multivariate Hotelling's T2 test determined that the population mean
betweenthe "cool" and
"hot"
(projected)
slides (for theL*,
a*, andb*coordinates)were statistically different in
every
case. This implies that the slideshad
0.478
0.476
0.474-(a)
Yellow
0.4698
hO.4694
0.469
0.4686
200
400
600
800
1000
0.348
0.346
(b)
Green
0
200
400
600
800
1000
1200
0.38-
0.376-
0.372-(c)
Dark
Gray
0
200
400
600
800
1000
1200
Seconds
0.386
FIG. 7. Simultaneous comparison of the x and y
chromaticity
coordinates forAb* 8
*
Cool
? Projected
(Hot)
20 minutes
i?Na%.
10<S-C\a$^>^
5vT^^Oa^2
K 20*15
*10
*5
-3 -2 -1 C) 1
Aa*
FIG. 8. Color shift (inCIELAB Aa* and
Ab*)
as the magenta slide isrepeatedly
heated up in the projector then allowed to cool. Both groups of measurementsshow a shiftinthe +b*
direction
(yellowing
staining) whichis observed forall other colors. (The original BYKCSdata,
before any
projection, is the standard(0,0).)
The
(b*,
a*) coordinates were also plotted for all eight colorssimultaneously
in Fig. 10. Plot(a)
representsthe overall effect ofprojection. Nosingle direction of color shift occurs. The coordinates
demonstrate
atransformation toward lower chroma, however sometimes
relatively large hue
6-1
2
-2- -6-104
Cyan
Projected0
Projected 6-Ab* 2- -2- -6-10 V +f
2
4
6
-4-20
6
6-Red
2-t
-2-Projected -6: 10- ' iMagenta
-40
t ' r
2--2: -6--10
6
2
-2 -6 -10\
+ + ProjectedYellow
-40
A
a*/
Green
Projected *4-20246
a 0 Projected6:
f
+ 2-+ -2--6:Blue
10-i -i i
0
FIG. 9. Comparison of the color shift between hot
(projected)
and ambient temperature(cool)
slides. The arrowsdepict
the change over timedue
to film
100-(a)
During
Projection50-1
t>* 0
-50-1
-100-A
/P
^
-50 -25
V
25 50
100
50
(b)
After Projection(degradation)
*
Original
*
After 20
minb* o
-50
-100
-50 -25 25 50
FIG. 10. Comparison ofthe color change after 20 minutes of projection. Notice
that the colors of the slides after projection
(b)
have ALL become more yellow(due to
degradation),
while the dominant shift for the projected slides(a)
istowardlowerchromaticity.
In comparison, Fig. 10
(b)
describes the change causedby
degradation inThese slides have all become more yellow. The magnitude of the change is
smaller than that induced
by
projection and incontrast, all colors shiftedonly
in the positiveb*direction.Table II summarizes the
AEa]-,
after 5 and 20 minutes of projection. Thevisual change due to thermochromism couldbe quite large (for example, green
was about 10
AEaj-,*
units) and at
times,
was reduced slightly upon furtherprojection. The degradation effect,
however,
appeared to be consistent overtime: with increased projection, the colors shifted from the original to the
yellow, positiveb* direction. This experiment was repeated withslides in open
Table II. Spectral differences (in terms of AEab*) due to
staining
(cool)
vs.thermochromism
(hot)
using the original cool (roomtemperature)
slide as thestandard. The differences were calculated from "uncorrected"
/
"regressioncorrected" spectra.
SlideColor Amountof
projection
Originalto
Cool
OriginaltoHot
Glass Mounts
OriginaltoHot Clear Mounts
Red 5minutes 1.4 3.9
/
3.7 3.2/
3.020minutes 2.4 3.4
/
3.2Green 5minutes 0.36 10.6
/
10.3 9.1/
9.020minutes 1.41 9.6
/
9.3Blue 5 minutes 1.8 6.3
/
6.1 6.3/
6.220minutes 3.8 8.7
/
8.5Cyan 5minutes 1.1 5.1
/
4.9 5.8/
5.620minutes 3.3 5.0
/
4.6Magenta 5minutes 1.9 5.0
/
4.7 5.4/
5.220minutes 4.1 7.2
/
7.0Yellow 5minutes 0.78 4.5
/
4.1 5.3/
5.020minutes 2.34 4.2
/
3.8Dark
Gray
5 minutes 1.3 2.6/
2.5 2.3/
2.220 minutes 2.8 2.9
/
2.7Light
Gray
5 minutes 1.3 0.41/
0.33 0.45/
0.392.3. Film Recorder
Calibration
2.3.1. Experiment 3:
Characteristic
Vectors
As discussed
before,
it isnecessary
toknow
the absorption properties of each dye in order to derive the tristimulus matches. While these curves areavailable from the manufacturer for cool measurements, another method was
needed to derive absorptivities for projected slides (since upon projection slides
change in color). This was accomplished using principal component analysis (with SYSTAT software) from measurements ofthree sets of
ramp
data. Single-color-slides created on the Solitaire-16 film recorder were chosen to produceelevensamplesforeachseries
ranging
from fullcolor(yellow,
magenta,or cyan) to white (no dye). The slides wereplaced in clear mounts and measuredon the BYKCS thenprojectedandmeasured with thePR703A. The data (400 to 700 nm in 10 nmintervals)
were convertedto density.1 The firsteigenvectorfor eachsetof ramps was used to model the dye absorptivity. This vector explained a
minimum of 99.8% variance in all cases.
Consequently,
the model provided areasonable estimateforthedye
density
curves.For comparison purposes, the predicted dye absorptivities for the hot and
cool vectors were normalized
by
peakwavelength. The eigenvectors are plotted in Fig. 11. The curveshapeofthe "hot"yellow and cyandyes are narrower thanthe
"cool"
dyes. More
importantly,
the "hot" magenta dyedensity
shiftedhypsochromically
10nm.1
(a)Yellow
400 500 600 700
>-,
l-
0.5-
0-(b) Magenta
CO
__:
CD
Q
"O CD N
(Xi
E
400 500
(c) Cyan
600 700
400 500 600
Wavelength
700
FIG. 11. 'Projected' dye absorptivities. Normalized
(first)
eigenvectorsderived
The shape of these new curves,
however,
seemed too pointed, so a secondset of eigenvectors was generated from a
database
of 60 colors. Anotherprincipal component analysis
using
an 'equimax' rotation was used to estimatethree eigenvectors (which corresponded to the three dye absorptivities). As
stated
by
Berns,
combining
the two sets of vectors through multiple linearregression
(thereby
minimizing
sum of squares error), produces the best basisvectors when usedforprediction ofdyeconcentrations.
2.3.2. Experiment 4: Tristimulus
Matching
2.3.2.1.
Slide
MeasurementAs
previously
stated, themodelfor thefilmrecorder requires exposure andmeasurement of a factorial design of red, green, and blue ramps. This was
accomplished
by
keeping
two digital counts at 96 and varying the third colorfrom0 to255 insteps of32. Aset of9 grayswas also preparedinorder tomodel
the tone reproduction. Measurements of previous slides made from the same
digital counts were problematic. About 10% of the slides were too dark to get
spectral data with the PR703A. Inorder to overcome this
hurdle,
simultaneousmeasurements of the slides with the PR703A and LMT Colormeter were made.
Slides which were too dark to integrate with the spectroradiometer, were
projectedforatleast75 seconds(thetime requiredforthe thermochromic shiftto
occur) before LMT tristimulus readings were recorded.
Using
the availablespectraldataandtheLMTtristimulusvalues,a
relationship
wasderived,
by
Roy
Berns,
between the calculated tristimulus values from the PR703A and the LMTXPR703A
=0-184 + (96.743 *XLMT)
+(2.325
*YLMT)
YPR703A
=0-157+ (101.627*YLMT)
-(1.123
*ZLMT)
(1)
ZPR703A
= (0781*XLMT)
+(50.441 *ZLMT)
2.3.2.2.
Modeling
Design
For the final model, a simpler experimental design was used. Slides were
created with two colors surrounded
by
agray
having
a reflectance ofapproximately
20%. Theseslides were shapedsimilarly
to the images employedin the paired-comparison psychophysical experiments of Phase II. A series of9
grays (from 0 to 255 in steps of
32)
was exposedtwice,
created on both sides ofthe slides. Interimage slides were not prepared.
Instead,
two digital countswere kept at 0 while the third varied from
64, 128, 192,
and 255 (producing:dark red, dark green, and dark
blue;
to full color). Measurements were madewith the LMT. In order to improve variability in the
data,
the slide projectorwas connectedtoavoltage regulator.
First,
the tristimulus values were corrected to estimate tristimulus valuesfrom spectral data. Next a tristimulus
matching
algorithm was calculated inorder to predict concentrations2. The 2
observer was used, with the projector
light source as the
illuminant,
and the transmittance of the Dmin was used toestimate the transmittance ofthe substrate. While the substrate is
actually only
the gelatin, the Dminis areasonablefirstapproximation.
2 The tristimulus
2.3.2.3.
Results: Evaluation
ofModel
Accuracy
The tristimulus
matching
algorithm was modeledwith twosets ofdata:1)
only
thegray
slides, and2)
the nine grays as well as the three ramps of cyan,magenta, and yellow. Measured and predicted concentrations of a separate test
set of colorswere usedto calculateAEab*'s. Themore complicatedmatrix, using
the interimage effects (while
maintaining
thegray
balance),
produced betterresults.
Table III. Comparison of two film recorder models. AEg]-,* of test slide colors using
only
agray
balancemodel orincluding
interimageeffects.For
ALL Color Slides
For
GRAY Slides
Average Range Average Range
Model from Grays 7.66 1.32- 17.44 1.74 0.42
-2.58
Model
Including
Interimage Effects 5.73 1.05
- 10.78 1.67
1.33-2.13
2.3.3. Experiment5:
Image
Variability
A wide variety of factors affect the
ability
toreliably
create an image.Physical limitations of the film recorder,
processing
variability, and imagedegradation can cause actual images to deviate from the predicted colors using
2.3.3.1.
Gray Variability
As
previously
stated,interimage
effects were evaluatedby
usinga factorialdesign,
of red, green, andblue
ramps. This was accomplishedby
keeping
twodigital counts at 96 and
varying
the third color from 0 to 255 in steps of 32. Atone pointin
modeling
thefilmrecorder,eachramp
wascreated ona single slide.Inall,a series of9slides
(red,
green,blue,
and2gray
ramps, aDminand3slidescontaining miscellaneous colors for model
testing)
were created three times.These slides were processed at the same
time,
buteach series was measured ondifferent days. The Dmin slide was used to normalize the tristimulus values
measured for all the other slides. On four ramps, a patch with a
gray
section(withequal digitalcounts of96 forthe red, green,and
blue)
wasexposed.Examination of the coefficient of variation (the standard deviation divided
by
the mean), allows a comparison between normalized values of differentmagnitude. Several combinations of the
gray
digital counts exhibited 5 to 10times the variability as the other colors: 0-0-0 (Cv =
11.8%),
64-64-64 (Cv =17.4%),
and 96-96-96 (Cv = 10.0%). Thevariability of the 96-96-96 slide was
examined in greater
detail,
since it was present on six different slides, in thesame position. If the
daily
populations (6 samples/day) of the normalizedtristimulus values are evaluated, some minor changes are observed (see
Fig
12).However,
when the data are reevaluatedby
the colorramp
of the slide(red,
green,
blue,
or either gray), the variability for the'color'
is
usually
muchsmallerand more
importantly,
different from other colors (Fig. 13). The slide-to-slide('color'
ramp) differences are dueto thephysicallimitationofthe CRTinthe film
neighboring
pixels. Theday-to-day
changes are a combination ofexposure andmeasurement
differences.
This will bediscussed
in greater detail in the nextsection.
Y_Variable = NORMALIZED 'X' TSV
0.027 +
0.026 +
0.025 +
0.024 +
0.023 +
0.022
0.021 +
0.02 +
0.019 +
0.018 +
+ +
* j *
+ +
r__+__*
+ +
2
DAY
FIG. 12. Populations of 96-96-96
gray
patches separatedby
DAY ofY_Variable = NORMALIZED 'X'
TSV
0.027 +
0.026 +
0.025 +
0.024 +
0.023
0.022 +
0.021 +
0.02 +
0.019 + + +
0.018 +
* *
*_4._*
--+ + H + + +
--Blue Grayl Gray2 Green Group3 Red
SLIDE
FIG. 13. Populationsof96-96-96
gray
patches sortedby
thesurrounding
COLOR2.3.3.2. Precision
Study
The purpose of this
study
was to anticipate the amount of variability thatcouldbe expected when film was exposed and processed over time. A set of 8
colors was exposed four times on three different rolls of film over three days.
Each roll was also processed on adifferent day. The colors were chosen
by
theiranticipated positionin CIELAB space (based upon previous experience with the
LUT used). A color fromeach quadrant in
CIELAB,
as well as four grays wereused. For each rollof
film,
theeight colorswererandomized (and theorder wasdifferenteachtime).
Average tristimulus values of each slide (measured with the
BYKCS,
calculated for illuminant
A,
and the 2 observer), were used to determine the"reference"
color from which AE^'s were calculated. For all 95 samples, the
mean AE^* was
only
0.46 with a range of 0.44 to 0.48 units per day. (Oneoutlier with a AEab* > 4 was removed, a gray with a digital count of 150). The
colors which seemed most variable werethemiddle grays. A
gray
of150 digitalcounts had a meanAEab*of0.99 (maximumof 2.03). A
gray
of 200 had a meanof
0.65,
withamaximum of1.21.Another issue was the effect of internal calibration of the film recorder.
These slides were randomized with a known sequence and calibrated
every
6slides. Aplot ofthe maximum and minimumAE^* values over time (indicated
by
the slide number on the roll offilm)
was used to determine ifthe calibrationhadanoticable effect. Asshownin Fig.
14,
the colorchanges appeartobe cyclic.2.3.3.3.
Processing Variability
The
variability
due toprocessing
changes is great. In order to minimizethese effects,the same photofinisher (witha goodreputation for
quality
control)was always used. Upon special request, this vendor (the RTT
Processing
Lab)
also supplied us with the process control strips used to monitor each batch of
film developed for this project. Adip-and-dunk
processing
style was used. Thefilm was placed on the same hanger as the control
strip
(to ensure a reasonableestimateofthe actual chemicalbehavior ofthefilmwe received). Thedensities
2
1.8
1.6
* 1.4
W 1.2
i
5
0-8O
0.60.4
0.2
0
DP
B
A?
" |'ii
H I I I I-m in n o\ r
i.o_ot>a\rHcn_n--^a-\><
,-h,<,i,i,ICNCNCNCNC^JCO
Slide Sequence
Minimum
a Maximum
FIG. 14. Change in AEab* with position on a roll of film (recalibrated
every
6of three steps:
3, 5,
and 7 on these control strips (chosen to reflect visuallynoticeable
densities)
were monitored using the status A filter set on a MacBethmodel TD903 densitometer. Figure 15 provides a
history
of the change in steps5. The dateofthecontrol
strip corresponding
to the slides usedformodeling
theSolitaire film recorder and for creation of the samples used in the D65
psychophysicalexperiments are marked onthe plot. Alsomarked on the graph,
arethecontrol strips oftheslides usedforthe precision study.
Precision
-3
to
4-Q
Film Recorder
Modeling
D65 Experiments
Date
The random fluctations in the
processing
(as exhibitedby
thedensity
measurements) make accurate sample preparation
extremely
difficult. There isno way to control apriori t