Potentiation rather than distraction in a trace fear conditioning procedure

Download (0)

Full text

(1)

Contents lists available atScienceDirect

Behavioural

Processes

j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e :w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c a t e / b e h a v p r o c

Potentiation

rather

than

distraction

in

a

trace

fear

conditioning

procedure

M.A.

Pezze,

H.J.

Marshall,

H.J.

Cassaday

SchoolofPsychology,UniversityofNottingham,UnitedKingdom

a

r

t

i

c

l

e

i

n

f

o

Articlehistory:

Received10December2015 Receivedinrevisedform5April2016 Accepted5April2016

Availableonline6April2016

Keywords: Traceconditioning

Conditionedemotionalresponse Attention

Distractor Potentiation

a

b

s

t

r

a

c

t

Traceconditioningproceduresaredefinedbytheintroductionofatraceintervalbetweenconditioned stimulus(CS,e.g.noiseorlight)offsetandunconditionedstimulus(US,e.g.footshock).Theintroduction ofanadditionalstimulusasadistractorhasbeensuggestedtoincreasetheattentionaldemandsofthe taskandtoextendtheusefulnessofthebehaviouralmodel.InExperiment1,theCSwasnoiseandthe distractorwasprovidedbyanintermittentlight.InExperiment2,theCSwaslightandthedistractorwas providedbyanintermittentnoise.Inbothexperiments,theintroductionofa10straceintervalweakened associativelearningcomparedwiththatseenina0sdelayconditionedgroup.However,therewasno consistentevidenceofdistraction.Onthecontrary,inExperiment1,associativelearningwasstronger (inbothtraceanddelayconditionedgroups)forratsconditionedalsointhepresenceoftheintermittent light.InExperiment2,therewasnosucheffectwhentherolesofthestimuliwerereversed.Theresultsof Experiment2didhoweverconfirmtheparticularsalienceofthenoisestimulus.Thefindingofincreased associativelearningdependentonsalienceisconsistentwitharousal-mediatedeffectsonassociative learning.

©2016TheAuthors.PublishedbyElsevierB.V.ThisisanopenaccessarticleundertheCCBYlicense (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).

1. Introduction

Traceconditioningproceduresaredefinedbytheintroduction ofatraceintervalbetweenconditionedstimulus(CS,e.g.noise) offsetandunconditionedstimulus(US,e.g.foodorfootshock)onset (Kamin,1965).Thecharacteristicresult−ofreducedconditioning inconsequenceoftemporaldiscontiguity−canbedemonstrated inavarietyofPavlovianconditioningprocedures(bothappetitive andaversive)butaversiveprocedureshavebeenmuchmorewidely adopted,bothbecauseacquisitionisrapidandtheneuralcircuitry necessarytobasicfearconditioningiswelldocumented.

Theabilitytobridgetimedelaystoshowassociativelearningina traceconditioningprocedureallowsanimalstoassociatewhatgoes withwhat,whenpotentiallycausally-relatedeventsareseparated intime.Thus,asameasureofworkingmemory,trace condition-ingholdspromiseasabehaviouralassayforage-relatedmemory decline:itisimpairedinagedrabbits(GravesandSolomon,1985), rats(McEchronetal., 2004;Moyerand Brown,2006)andmice (Galvezetal.,2011;Kishimotoetal.,2001),aswellasinamouse modelofsenescence(Lopez-Ramosetal.,2012).

∗Correspondingauthor.

E-mailaddress:helen.cassaday@nottingham.ac.uk(H.J.Cassaday).

Inyoungeradultanimals,traceconditioninghasbeenshown to requirean intact hippocampusto process thetemporal gap betweentheCSandUS(McEchronetal.,1998;Weissetal.,1999; McEchronetal.,2000;Beylinetal.,2001;Quinnetal.,2002;Rogers andKesner,2006)and−asisthecasefortaskswhichmeasure declarative memory− seemstodependupon awareness(Clark andSquire,1998).Consistentwithknownprojectionsfrom hip-pocampus,medialprefrontalcortex(mPFC)hasalsobeenshown tobepartofthetraceconditioningnetwork.Comparingacrossa varietyoftraceconditioningpreparations,theemergingpattern seemstobearolefortheprelimbic(PL)sub-regionwhen mem-oryprocessesaredirectlyengaged,forexamplewhenretentionis tested(Runyanetal.,2004;Oswaldetal.,2008,2010),when neu-ronalactivityisexaminedduringarelativelylongtraceinterval (GilmartinandMcEchron,2005)orwhenlongerCSdurations com-poundthememoryload(McLaughlinetal.,2002).Incontrast,there isevidencetosuggestthattheanteriorcingulate(AC)sub-region isimportantforearlieracquisition-relatedprocesses (Kronforst-CollinsandDisterhoft,1998;Weibleetal.,2003,2000;Kalmbach etal.,2009;Hattorietal.,2014).Thisdistinctionmayrelatetothe roleofACinattentionalprocessesand−consistentwiththis inter-pretation−excitoxiclesionsoftheACsub-regionofmPFCwere reportedtoreducetraceconditioninginamousefearconditioning

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2016.04.003

(2)

procedurewhichwassensitivetotheeffectsofanexperimental distractorstimulus(Hanetal.,2003).

Ineye-blinktraceconditioningprocedures,humanparticipants’ abilitytoreportontheCS-USrelationshipissimilarlyimpairedby concurrentdistraction,and thisfindinghasalsobeenconfirmed usingatracefearconditioningprocedure,inthiscasewithafinger shockUSCarteretal.(2003).Thislatterstudywasdesignedtobe analogoustotheHanetal.rodentstudy,thoughthenatureofthe experimentaldistractionwasdifferent.Carteretal.(2003)useda concurrentn-backtask,whichrequiredparticipantstotrack pre-viouslypresenteddigitsinalistofnumbers,bywayofadistractor intendedtocompeteforworkingmemorycapacity.Aswasthecase intheHanetal.(2003)mouseconditioningstudy,distractorstimuli weresimilarlyfoundtointerferewithtracefearconditioning,delay conditioningbeingmuchmoreresilienttotheeffectsofdistraction (Carteretal.,2003).

Thus, it has been argued that the use of a distractoris an importantprocedural modificationin order tomodel the puta-tiveattentionalroleofACinataskwithdemonstratedsensitivity toattentionalparametersandhightranslationalrelevancetoour understandingofnormalhumanageing.Moreover,itfollowsthat increasedattentionalloadmaybeacontributingfactorintheevent traceconditioningdeficitsaredemonstratedinrodentmodels,at leasttotheextentthatthesedependonattentionalprocesses medi-atedbytheAC(Pezzeetal.,2016).

In a series of trace conditioningexperiments using rat fear conditioning procedures, we have routinely used an extended backgroundstimulus(NormanandCassaday,2003;Horsleyand Cassaday,2007;Grimond-Billa etal., 2008;Nelsonet al.,2011; Pezzeetal.,2016).Thiswasprovidedbyacontinuouslyflashing lightpresentedforthefulldurationoftheconditioningsessionand hasbeenintendedtoprovideanexperimentalcontextratherthan adistractorstimulus.ThedistractorstimulususedbyHanetal. (2003)wasalsoprovidedbyaflashinglight,differentonlyinits temporalproperties.Therefore,sincedistractionisofboth theo-reticalandpracticalimportance−toboththeinterpretationand demonstrationoftraceconditioningimpairments−weadapted ourexistingfearconditioningprocedureinanattempttoestablish areliabledistractorsuitableforuseinrats.

Itmustbenotedthatundersomeexperimentalcircumstances theintroductionofextraneousstimuliisalreadyknowntoresultin potentiationratherthandistraction(DurlachandRescorla,1980; Pearceetal.,1981;Rescorla,1982;HallandHoney,1993).Itwasnot ourobjectivetoaddtothisbodyofknowledge.Ratherthepresent studysoughttoexplorethefeasibilityofadaptingapublished dis-tractorprocedure,inorder(inthelongerterm)tofurtherexamine theroleofACinworkingmemory.Thisbehaviouralworkwasdone inaratratherthanamousemodelandusingadifferentvariantof tracefearconditioning(suppressionoflickingratherthanfreezing), asperanumberofearlierstudiesconductedtoexaminethe neu-ropharmacologicalsubstratesoftraceconditioning(Normanand Cassaday,2003;HorsleyandCassaday,2007;Grimond-Billaetal., 2008;Nelsonetal.,2011;Pezzeetal.,2016).

2. Methods

2.1. Animals

Ineachoftwoexperiments,48experimentallynaïveadultmale Wistarrats(CharlesRiver,UK)werecagedingroupsof4in indi-viduallyventilatedcages(IVCs),ona12:12hlight/darkcyclewith foodandwateradlibitum.Cageswerecleanedouttwiceperweek andcardboardtubesandnestingmaterialswereprovidedas envi-ronmentalenrichment.Theratswerehandledforapproximately 5minperdayfor1weekandthenatmeanweight199g(range

168–224g)inExperiment1and218g(range193–246g)in Exper-iment2wereplacedonwaterdeprivationimmediatelypriorthe conditioningprocedures.Onerat(inExperiment1)washumanely killedforanunrelatedreason,ontheadviceoftheNamed Veteri-narySurgeon.Allprocedureswerecarriedoutinaccordancewith theUnitedKingdom(UK)AnimalsScientificProceduresAct1986, ProjectLicensenumberPPL40/3716,whichensuresfullcompliance withtheEUDirective2010/63/EUforanimalexperiments.

2.2. Behaviouralconditioningapparatus

Four identical fully automated conditioning boxes, housed withinsound-attenuatingcasescontainingventilationfans (Cam-bridgeCognition, Cambridge, UK), were used. Theinner condi-tioningboxwallsconsistedofplainsteel(25cm×25cm×22cm high)withaPlexiglasdoor(27cm×21cmhigh),atthefront.The floorwasashockgridwithsteelbars1cmapartand1cmabove thelipofa7cmdeepsawdusttray.Awaterspoutwasmounted ononewall.Thespoutwas5cmabovethefloorandconnected toalickometersuppliedbya pump.Lickswereregisteredbya breakin thephotobeam withinthespout,which alsotriggered waterdeliveryof0.05mlperlick.Thewaterspoutwasilluminated whenwaterwasavailable.Aloudspeakerforthepresentationof auditorystimuliwassetintheroof.InExperiment1,a5smixed frequencycontinuousnoisesetat80dBservedastheCSandthe distractorwasanintermittentlight providedbythethree wall-mounteddome-shapedstimuluslightsandthehouselightsetto flashintermittently(130mson/off,at8lx,for3sdurationwithan interstimulusintervalrandomlychosenfrom5,10,15or20s).In Experiment2,a5sflashinglightservedastheCS(inthiscase pro-videdbythethreewallmountedstimuluslightsandthehouselight flashing(500mson/off,at8lx)andthedistractorwasan intermit-tentnoise(130mson/offfor3s,setat80dBwithaninterstimulus intervalsequencerandomlychosenfrom5,10,15or20s). Foot-shockof1sdurationand 1mAintensityprovidedtheUCS.This wasdeliveredthroughthegridfloorbyaconstantcurrentshock generator(pulsedvoltage: outputsquarewave 10mson, 80ms off,370Vpeakundernoloadconditions,MISACSystems, New-bury,UK).StimuluscontrolanddatacollectionwasbyanAcorn ArchimedesRISCcomputerprogrammedinBasicwithadditional interfacingusinganArachnidextension(CambridgeCognition).

2.3. Behaviouralconditioningprocedure

Waterdeprivationwasintroduced1daypriortoshapingand allratsreceived1hofadlibitumaccesstowaterintheirhomecage atthesametimeeachday,inadditiontoaccesstowaterinthe conditioningapparatusonalltheexperimentaldaysexcept con-ditioning.Thestagesofthetraceconditioningprocedurewereas follows:

2.3.1. Pre-conditioningtoestablishbaselinelickresponse

(3)

2.3.2. Conditioningwithfootshock

Conditioningwasconductedfollowingpre-training.Nowater wasavailablewithintheboxandthewaterspoutwasnot illumi-nated.Therewere2conditioningtrialsinwhichtheUCSfootshock wasdeliveredata0sor10straceintervalfollowingtermination ofthe5sCS.ThefirstpairingofCSandUCSwaspresentedafter 5minhadelapsed,andthesecondpairingwas5minafterthefirst, followedbyafurther5minleftintheapparatus.Intheabsenceof drinking,therewerenobehaviouralmeasurestorecord.Inboth experiments,thedistractorwasprovidedbyanintermittent back-groundstimulus(lightinExperiment1,noiseinExperiment2).For animalsinthedistractorcondition,thepresentationofthe distrac-torstarted1minbeforethefirstCS-UCSpairingandended1min afterthefinalCS-UCSpresentation.

2.3.3. Reshapingafterfootshock

Onthedayfollowingconditioning,animalswerereshaped fol-lowingthesameprocedureasinpre-trainingsessions.Thiswas donein orderto re-establishdrinkingafterconditioning. Addi-tionally,thereshapinglatenciesprovidedameasureofcontextual conditioningasreflectedinsuppressiontothecontextualcues pro-videdbytheexperimentalchambers.

2.3.4. Conditionedsuppressiontests

On thedayfollowingreshaping,theanimals wereplaced in theconditioningboxesandunderwent anextinction testtothe CS.Waterwasavailablethroughoutthetestandthewaterspout wasilluminated.Oncetheanimalshadmade50licks,theCSwas presentedfor15min.Thelatencytomake50licksintheabsence oftheCS (theA period,timedfromthefirstlick madein each box)providedameasureof anyindividualvariation inbaseline lickresponding.Thiswascomparedwiththetimetakento com-plete50licksfollowingCSonset(Bperiod)inasuppressionratio (A/(A+B))toassessthelevelofconditioningtotheCS,adjustedfor anyindividualvariationindrinkrate.Conditionedsuppressionwas alsomeasuredasthenumberoflicksmadeduringthefirst1minof CSpresentation.Asecondextinctiontestmeasuredsuppressionto thedistractorstimulusinthesameway.Theextinctiontestswere runinacounterbalancedorder.

2.4. Experimentaldesignandstatisticalanalysis

Inboth experiments, therewere2 conditioninggroups con-ditioned with or without a concurrently presented distractor stimulusina2×2factorialdesignatlevels0or10strace inter-valandwithorwithoutthedistractor(n=12/group).Statistical analyseswereperformedusinganalysisofvariance(ANOVA).The pre-conditioning and reshaping dependent variables were lick latencies,aswellasthenumberoflicksmadeinthefirst1min ofthesession.Thetestdependentvariableswerethesuppression ratios,aswellasthenumberoflicksmadeduringthefirst1mintest presentation.Thesamevariableswereanalysedinthesamewayfor thetestsofsuppressiontotheCSandthedistractorstimulus. How-ever,examinationoftheeffectsoftraceintervalonsuppressionto thedistractorwasnecessarilyrestrictedtothoseratsinthegroups actuallypresentedwithadistractor.Finally,acombinedanalysis comparedconditioningasafunctionofnoiseversuslighttargetCS identityandlightversusnoisedistractoridentity.Non-significant effectsonbaselinerespondingarenotreported.

3. Results

3.1. Experiment1:noiseCSwithintermittentlightdistractor

Therewasa clear effectof traceonboth suppression ratios tothenoiseCS,F(1,43)=8.573,p=0.005,aswellasonthe

mea-sureoflearningprovidedbythenumberoflicksmadeinthefirst 1minoftest,F(1,43)=8.160,p=0.007.Fig.1panelsAandBshow that,asexpected,ratsconditionedthatthe0straceintervalwere moresuppressedthanthoseconditionedatthe10strace inter-val.Countertoprediction,therewasnoevidencethatthelight distractormoderatedconditioningoverthetraceintervalinthat theinteractionbetweentraceanddistractorwasnotsignificantfor eithermeasure,maximumF(1,43)=0.004,p=0.949(Fig.1A). Con-firmingthatthelightdistractorwasnotwithouteffect,therewas amaineffectonthesuppressionratiomeasureofconditioningto thenoiseCS,F(1,43)=4.786,p=0.034(Fig.1A).Thisarosebecause ratspresentedwiththeflashinglightdistractorduring condition-ingwereoverallmoresuppressedthanthoseconditionedwithout the‘distractor’. Althoughnon-significant,the samepattern was reflectedinthelicksmeasureofconditionedsuppression(Fig.1B). Direct tests of conditioning to the light distractor stimulus showed no evidence of conditioning to this stimulus in that therewasnodifferenceinsuppressionratiosforratsconditioned withorwithouttheintermittentlightpresentedasadistractor, F(1,43)=1.131,p=0.294.Fig.1Cshowsthatthemeanswerevery comparable(overallSR=0.378withand0.340without).Giventhe lackofdifferenceinsuppressionforratsconditionedwithand with-outthedistractor, thefact thattheobservedsuppressionratios werebelow0.5 canbeattributedtounconditionedsuppression andconfirmsthesalienceofthelightdistractorstimulus.Thesame patternwasreflectedinthelicksmeasureofconditioned suppres-sion,onlytheinteractionapproachedsignificance forthemin1 licksmeasure,F(1,43)=3.758,p=0.059(Fig.1D).However,ANOVA restrictedtothoseratsactuallypresentedwiththelight distrac-toratconditioningconfirmedthattherewasalsonoeffectofthe traceintervalatwhichthenoiseCShadbeenpresentedoneither measure of conditioningto thelight, maximum F(1,21)=0.166, p=0.688.

3.2. Experiment2:lightCSwithintermittentnoisedistractor

Theeffectoftraceonthesuppressionratiostothelightwas marginal,F(1,44)=3.824,p=0.057(Fig.2A),buttherewasaclear effectoftraceonthenumberoflicksmadeinthefirst1mintest presentation,F(1,44)=6.116,p=0.017(Fig.2B).Therewasno sig-nificanteffectofthepresenceorabsenceoftheintermittentnoise distractoronconditioningtothelightCS,maximumF(1,44)=3.246, p=0.078,fortheinteractionbetweentraceanddistractorforthe 1mindrinkingattest.

Directtestsofconditioningtothenoisedistractorshowedno effect, F(1,44)=1.836, p=0.182, for the suppression ratio mea-sure(SR=0.123withpriorconditioningandSR=0.171withoutany priorconditioning;Fig.2C).Thefactthattheobservedsuppression ratios were relatively low irrespective of whetherthe distrac-torhadbeenpresentatconditioningmeansthatitspresentation resultedinunconditionedsuppressionandsuggeststhatthe inter-mittentnoisestimuluswaslikelyparticularlysalient.Althoughthe suppressionratiomeasurewasinsensitivetoitseffects,rats con-ditionedinthepresenceofthenoisedistractorshowedreduced drinkingduringthefirst1mintestpresentationofsamethe inter-mittentnoisestimulus,F(1,44)=6.539,p=0.014(Fig.2D).ANOVA restrictedtothoseratsactuallypresentedwiththenoisedistractor atconditioningconfirmedthattherewas,however,noeffectofthe traceintervalatwhichthelightCShadbeenpresentedoneither measureof conditioningtothenoise, maximumF(1,22)=0.343, p=0.564.

3.3. Comparisonofexperiments1and2

(4)

0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5

Delay 0s Trace 10s

Suppr

essio

n

r

ao

0 40 80 120 160 200

Delay 0s Trace 10s

Min 1 lick

s

0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5

Delay 0s Trace 10s No distractor Distractor

0 40 80 120 160 200

Delay 0s Trace 10s

A

B

C

D

Fig.1. showssuppressionmeasuresofconditioningtothenoiseCSandlightdistractorinExperiment1.Fig.1AshowsthelevelofconditioningtothenoiseCSexpressed asmeansuppressionratio(calculatedasA/(A+B);whereAwasthetimetakentocomplete50lickspriortoCSpresentationandBwasthetimetakentocomplete50licks duringCSpresentation).Fig.1BshowsthelevelofconditioningtothenoiseCSexpressedasthenumberoflicksmadeduringthefirstminuteofCSpresentation.Theshaded histogramsshowhowrats’responsesweremoderatedbythepresence(darkgrey)orabsence(lightgrey)oftheintermittentlightdistractor.Fig.1Cshowsthesuppression ratiosupontestpresentationofthelightdistractor.Fig.1Dshowsthelevelofsuppressiontothelightexpressedasthenumberoflicksmadeduringthefirstminuteof presentation.Theshadedhistogramsshowhowrats’responsesdependedonpriorconditioning(darkgrey)orwereunconditioned(lightgrey).Errorbarsshowtwostandard errorsofthemeanforapproximatebetweengroupscomparisons.

0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5

Delay 0s Trace 10s

Suppr

essio

n

r

ao

0 40 80 120 160 200

Delay 0s Trace 10s

Min 1 lick

s

0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5

Delay 0s Trace 10s

No distractor

Distractor

0 40 80 120 160 200

Delay 0s Trace 10s

A

B

C

D

Fig.2. showssuppressionmeasuresofconditioningtothelightCSandnoisedistractorinExperiment2.Fig.2AshowsthelevelofconditioningtothelightCSexpressed asmeansuppressionratio(calculatedasA/(A+B);whereAwasthetimetakentocomplete50lickspriortoCSpresentationandBwasthetimetakentocomplete50licks duringCSpresentation).Fig.1BshowsthelevelofconditioningtothelightCSexpressedasthenumberoflicksmadeduringthefirstminuteofCSpresentation.Theshaded histogramsshowhowrats’responsesweremoderatedbythepresence(darkgrey)orabsence(lightgrey)oftheintermittentnoisedistractor.Fig.1Cshowsthesuppression ratiosupontestpresentationofthenoisedistractor.Fig.1Dshowsthelevelofsuppressiontothenoiseexpressedasthenumberoflicksmadeduringthefirstminuteof presentation.Theshadedhistogramsshowhowrats’responsesdependedonpriorconditioning(darkgrey)orwereunconditioned(lightgrey).Errorbarsshowtwostandard errorsofthemeanforapproximatebetweengroupscomparisons.

theSRmeasureofconditioningtothetargetCS,F(1,87)=10.927, p<0.001, aswellasfor 1stminlicks,F(1,87)=14.019,p<0.001. Therewasnoeffectofdistractoronthesemeasuresof condition-ingtotarget,eitheroverallorininteractionwithtrace,maximum F(1,87)=1.716,p=0.194.Therewasamaineffect ofCSidentity on the suppression ratio measure of learning, F(1,87)=23.243,

(5)

4. Discussion

Aswouldbeexpected,inbothexperiments,theintroductionof a10straceintervalweakenedassociativelearningcomparedwith thatseenina0sconditionedgroup.However,therewasno evi-denceofdistractioninthegroupsconditionedinthepresenceof eitheranintermittentlight(inExperiment1)oranintermittent noise(inExperiment2).Onthecontrary,inExperiment1, associa-tivelearningtothenoisewasstronger(andforbothtraceanddelay groups)forratsconditionedalsointhepresenceofthe intermit-tentlight.Asmightbeexpectedgiventhattheintermittentlight increasedratherthandecreasedconditioningtothenoise,there wasnoevidencetosuggestthatthelightacquiredanyassociative strengthofitsown.

Althoughbasedonaratfearconditioningprocedure,the pro-cedureadoptedinExperiment1wasbroadlysimilartothatused inthestudyofHanetal.(2003).Ofcoursetheremaybespecies differencesbetweenratsandmicewhichcanaccountforthese dis-crepantfindingsand(relatedly)theconditioningparametersused formicearequitedifferent(seebelow).

Onepossibilitywhich isimportanttoconsideris thatofthe modalitiesofthestimuliinuse.FollowingthepublishedHanetal. (2003)design,stimulusidentitywasnotcounterbalancedwithin Experiment1:thenoisewastheCSandtheintermittentlightthe notionaldistractor forall animals. Thus, takenin isolation, the resultsof Experiment1leaveopenthepossibilitythat interfer-encewithtraceconditioningmightdependonstimulusmodality and/orthelevelofarousalgenerated.Inappetitiverat condition-ingprocedures,effectsofstimulusmodalityhavepreviouslybeen foundtooutweighdifferencesduetotheinformationvalueofthe cueinrelationtodrugeffects(HorsleyandCassaday,2008). More-over,infearconditioningprocedures,effectsofstimulusmodality havebeenfoundtodependonstrainofrat(NormanandCassaday, 2004).Therefore,inExperiment2ofthepresentstudy,the stimu-lusroleswerereversed:anintermittentlightwastheCSandthe samenoisestimuluswasnowpresentedintermittentlyinthe back-ground,toprovidetheputativedistractor.InExperiment2,when therolesofthestimuliwerereversed,therewasnoeffectof gen-erallyincreasedconditioningtotheCSinthedistractorcondition. ThislackofeffectinExperiment2isconsistentwiththepotential importanceofstimulusmodality.Moreover,combinedanalysesof Experiments1and2,confirmedthattheidentityofthestimulus selectedasCSwasthemajordeterminantofthelevelofassociative learning,forbothtraceanddelayconditionedgroups.Nonetheless, theresultsofExperiment2dodemonstratetheparticularsalience oftheintermittentnoisestimulus.Thus,theeffectofmodalitycould ineffectbemediatedbysalience.

There was also a potentially important amodal difference betweenthestimuliwhichwaslikelytocontributetodifferences inperceivedsalience.TheauditoryCSusedinExperiment1was continuous(asperHan etal., 2003; and thetraceconditioning proceduresroutinelyusedinourlaboratory).However,continuous lightstimuliseemtobeofrelativelylowsalienceanditisinany casedifficulttomatchonthebasisofphysicalintensity(loudness versusbrightness)acrossmodalities.Wehavetypicallyused inter-mittentratherthancontinuouslightCSsinanattempttomatch thelevelofconditioningproducedbyalternativeauditoryversus lightCSsasfaraspossible,andanintermittentlightCSwasused inExperiment2.Thiswaspresentedwiththeflashingparameters usedpreviouslytoproduceconditioningratherthanthoseadopted whentheflashinglightwasintendedasadistractorinExperiment 1.Wesubmitthattheobserveddifferencesinconditioningwiththe auditoryversuslightCSscouldalsobeattributabletodifferencesin theamodalcharactersticsprovidedbytheirtemporaldifferences. However,thedifferenceintemporalparametersdoesnotseemto providethebestaccountofthepresence(inExperiment1)versus

absenceofpotentiation(in Experiment2)asthereshouldhave beengreaterpotentialforwithincompoundassociationsbasedon similarityinExperiment2(RescorlaandGillan,1980).

Thediscrepancybetweenthepresentresultandthemousefear traceconditioningvariantstillstandsinneedofexplanation.Itmust beacknowledgedthatwedidnotattemptafullreplicationofthe fearconditioningparametersusedbyHanetal.(2003)inmice. Insteadweadaptedaproceduredevelopedspecificallyforratsand whichhasalreadybeenusedinanumberoftracefear condition-ingstudies,inanattempttoestablishareliabledistractorsuitable foruseinrats.InthestudyconductedbyHanetal.(2003),the responsemeasurewasfreezingratherthanconditioned suppres-sionofamotivatedresponse(ashere).Accordingly,differencesin thebasicfearconditioningprocedureinuseincludedtheduration ofthepre-experimentallicktraining,thelengthoftheCS,tone fre-quencyandintensity,thenumberofconditioningtrialsand the shockintensity.

Therewerealsotwodifferencesinthedistractionaspectofthe procedure.First,inordertomakeitstemporalpropertiesmore dis-similarfromourstandardflashinglightCS(500mson/off)which hasprovenaneffectiveconditioningstimulusintherattrace con-ditioningprocedure,weincreasedthefrequencywithwhichthe intermittentdistractorwaspresented(130mson/offfor3s dura-tions,comparedwith250mson/offfor3sdurationsinHanetal., 2003).We cannotexcludethepossibilitythat a distractorwith temporalpropertiesmoresimilartothoseusedintheHanetal. (2003)wouldhavehadthedesiredeffectofdistractingfromrather thanenhancinglearningaboutthetrace-conditionedCS.The sec-onddifferencecanbeseenasastrengthinthat,inExperiment2, wealsocomparedtheeffectsofanintermittentnoisestimulusas a distractorfromthestandard flashing lightCS (500ms on/off) in anotherwise identicalprocedure. Thisis animportant com-parisonbecauseevenwithinspecies,effectsofstimulusmodality candependonstrain(NormanandCassaday,2004).The intermit-tentnoisestimuluswassimilarlyineffectiveasadistractor,though therewasnoevidenceforpotentiationeitherinExperiment2.

(6)

4.1. Conclusions

Overalltheresultssuggestthatthepresentationofanadditional cueduringtheconditioningsessioncanparadoxicallyimprove con-ditioningtothetargetCS,dependingontherelativesalienceofthe additionalcue.Inprinciple,thisfindingcouldreflectpotentiation basedon within-compound associations (Durlach and Rescorla, 1980;Pearceetal.,1981).However,potentiationwasseenonly inExperiment1whichusedacontinuousnoiseCSwhichwasmost dissimilartotheintermittentdistractor(RescorlaandGillan,1980). Moreover,therewasnoevidencethateitherthelightornoise dis-tractoracquiredassociativestrength andneitherwasthereany differencebetweentraceanddelayconditionedgroupsin Experi-ment1inwhichthelightdistractorincreasedassociativeleaning tothenoise.Thefindingthatthepresenceofsalientbackground cuescanincreaseassociativelearningisconsistentwith arousal-mediatedinterpretations.However,thenoisedistractorwhichwas moresalientthanthelighthadnosucheffect.Thusthelevelof arousalmaybecritical(YerkesandDodson,1908).

Acknowledgements

Thisresearchwassupportedbya grantfromtheBBSRC(ref. BB/K004980/1).WethankAndySmithfortechnicalsupportand assistancewithprogramming.

References

Beylin,A.V.,Gandhi,C.C.,Wood,G.E.,Talk,A.C.,Matzel,L.D.,Shors,T.J.,2001.The roleofthehippocampusintraceconditioning:temporaldiscontinuityortask difficulty?Neurobiol.Learn.Mem.76(3),447–461,http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/ nlme.2001.4039.

Carter,R.M.,Hofst ¨otter,C.,Tsuchiya,N.,Koch,C.,2003.Workingmemoryandfear conditioning.PNAS100(3),1399–1404,http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas. 0334049100.

Clark,R.E.,Squire,L.R.,1998.Classicalconditioningandbrainsystems:theroleof awareness.Science280(5360),77–81,http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.280. 5360.77.

Durlach,P.J.,Rescorla,R.A.,1980.Potentiationratherthanovershadowinginflavor aversionlearning:ananalysisintermsofwithin-compoundassociations.J. Exp.Psychol.Anim.B.6(2),175–187,http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.6. 2.175.

Galvez,R.,Cua,S.,Disterhoft,J.F.,2011.Age-relateddeficitsina

forebrain-dependenttask,trace-eyeblinkconditioning.Neurobiol.Aging32 (10),1915–1922,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.11.014. Gilmartin,M.R.,McEchron,M.D.,2005.Singleneuronsinthemedialprefrontal

cortexoftheratexhibittonicandphasiccodingduringtracefearconditioning. Behav.Neurosci.119(6),1496–1510,http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044. 119.6.1496.

Graves,C.A.,Solomon,P.R.,1985.Age-relateddisruptionoftracebutnotdelay classicalconditioningoftherabbit’snictitatingmembraneresponse.Behav. Neurosci.99(1),88–96,http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.99.1.88. Grimond-Billa,S.K.,Norman,C.,Bennett,G.W.,Cassaday,H.J.,2008.Selectively

increasedtraceconditioningundertheneurotensinagonistPD149163inan aversiveprocedureinwhichSR142948Awaswithoutintrinsiceffect.J. Psychopharmacol.22(3),290–299,http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ 0269881106081528.

Hall,G.,Honey,R.C.,1993.Poststimuluseventsindiscriminationlearningwith delayedreinforcement:roleofdistractionandimplicationsformarking.Learn. Motiv.24(3),242–254,http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/lmot.1993.1014.

Han,C.J.,O’Tuathaigh,C.M.,vanTrigt,L.,Quinn,J.J.,Fanselow,M.S.,Mongeau,R., Koch,C.,Anderson,D.J.,2003.Tracebutnotdelayfearconditioningrequires attentionandtheanteriorcingulatecortex.PNAS100(22),13087–13092, http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2132313100.

Hattori,S.,Yoon,T.,Disterhoft,J.F.,Weiss,C.,2014.Functionalreorganizationofa prefrontalcorticalnetworkmediatingconsolidationoftraceeyeblink conditioning.J.Neurosci34(4),1432–1445,http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/ JNEUROSCI.4428-13.2014.

Horsley,R.R.,Cassaday,H.J.,2007.Methylphenidatecanreduceselectivityin associativelearninginanaversivetraceconditioningtask.J.Psychopharmacol. 21(5),492–500,http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0269881106067381.

Horsley,R.R.,Cassaday,H.J.,2008.Amphetamineeffectsinappetitiveacquisition dependonthemodalityofthestimulusratherthanitsrelativevalidity.Pro. Neuropsychopharmacol.Biol.Psychiatry32(4),1057–1063,http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2008.02.005.

Kalmbach,B.E.,Ohyama,T.,Kreider,J.C.,Riusech,F.,Mauk,M.D.,2009.Interactions betweenprefrontalcortexandcerebellumrevealedbytraceeyelid

conditioning.Learn.Mem.16(1),86–95,http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/lm. 1178309.

Kamin,L.J.,1965.Temporalandintensitycharacteristicsoftheconditioned stimulus.In:Prokasy,W.F.(Ed.),ClassicalConditioning:ASymposium. Appleton-Century-Crofts,NewYork.

Kishimoto,Y.,Suzuki,M.,Kawahara,S.,Kirino,Y.,2001.Age-dependent impairmentofdelayandtraceeyeblinkconditioninginmice.Neuroreport12 (15),3349–3352,http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200110290-00040. Kronforst-Collins,M.A.,Disterhoft,J.F.,1998.Lesionsofthecaudalareaofrabbit

medialprefrontalcorteximpairtraceeyeblinkconditioning.Neurobiol.Learn. Mem.69(2),147–162,http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nlme.1997.3818.

Lopez-Ramos,J.C.,Jurado-Parras,M.T.,Sanfeliu,C.,Acuna-Castroviejo,D., Delgado-Garcia,J.M.,2012.LearningcapabilitiesandCA1-prefrontalsynaptic plasticityinamicemodelofacceleratedsenescence.Neurobiol.Aging33(3), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.04.005,627.e13–627.e26. McEchron,M.D.,Bouwmeester,H.,Tseng,W.,Weiss,C.,Disterhoft,J.F.,1998.

Hippocampectomydisruptsauditorytracefearconditioningandcontextual fearconditioningintherat.Hippocampus8(6),638–646,http://dx.doi.org/10. 1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)8:6<638:AID-HIPO6>3.0.CO;2-Q.

McEchron,M.D.,Tseng,W.,Disterhoft,J.F.,2000.Neurotoxiclesionsofthedorsal hippocampusdisruptauditory-cuedtraceheartrate(fear)conditioningin rabbits.Hippocampus10(6),739–751, http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1098-1063(2000)10:6<739:AID-HIPO1011>3.0.CO;2-I.

McEchron,M.D.,Cheng,A.Y.,Gilmartin,M.R.,2004.Tracefearconditioningis reducedintheagingrat.Neurobiol.Learn.Mem.82(2),71–76,http://dx.doi. org/10.1016/j.nlm.2004.06.002.

McLaughlin,J.,Skaggs,H.,Churchwell,J.,Powell,D.A.,2002.Medialprefrontal cortexandPavlovianconditioning:traceversusdelayconditioning.Behav. Neurosci.116(1),37–47,http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0735-7044.116.1.37. Moyer,J.R.,Brown,T.H.,2006.Impairedtraceandcontextualfearconditioningin

agedrats.Behav.Neurosci.120(3),612–624, http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.120.3.612.

Nelson,A.J.D.,Thur,K.E.,Spicer,C.,Marsden,C.A.,Cassaday,H.J.,2011. Catecholaminergicdepletioninnucleusaccumbensenhancestrace conditioning.Adv.Med.Sci.56(1),71–79, http://dx.doi.org/10.2478/v10039-011-0014-2.

Norman,C.,Cassaday,H.J.,2003.Amphetamineincreasesaversiveconditioningto diffusecontextualstimuliandtoadiscretetracestimuluswhenconditionedat higherfootshockintensity.J.Psychopharmacol.17(1),67–76,http://dx.doi. org/10.1177/0269881103017001701.

Norman,C.,Cassaday,H.J.,2004.CERtodiscreteandcontextualstimuli:effectsof stimulusmodalitydependonstrainofrat.Physiol.Behav.82(4),611–619, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.05.013.

Oswald,B.B.,Maddox,S.A.,Powell,D.A.,2008.Prefrontalcontroloftraceeyeblink conditioninginrabbits:roleinretrievaloftheCR?Behav.Neurosci.122(4), 841–848,http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.122.4.841.

Oswald,B.B.,Maddox,S.A.,Tisdale,N.,Powell,D.A.,2010.Encodingandretrieval aredifferentiallyprocessedbytheanteriorcingulateandprelimbiccortices:a studybasedontraceeyeblinkconditioningintherabbit.Neurobiol.Learn. Mem.93(1),37–45,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2009.08.001.

Pearce,J.M.,Nicholas,D.J.,Dickinson,A.,1981.Thepotentiationeffectduringserial conditioning.Q.J.Exp.Psychol.B.33(3),159–179,http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ 14640748108400820.

Pezze,M.A.,Marshall,H.J.,Domonkos,A.,Cassaday,H.J.,2016.Effectsofdopamine D1modulationoftheanteriorcingulatecortexinafearconditioning

procedure.Prog.Neuropsychopharmacol.Biol.Psychiatry65,60–67,http://dx. doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2015.08.015.

Quinn,J.J.,Oommen,S.S.,Morrison,G.E.,Fanselow,M.S.,2002.Post-training excitotoxiclesionsofthedorsalhippocampusattenuateforwardtrace, backwardtrace,anddelayfearconditioninginatemporallyspecificmanner. Hippocampus12(4),495–504,http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hipo.10029. Rescorla,R.A.,Gillan,D.J.,1980.Ananalysisofthefacilitativeeffectofsimilarityon

second-orderconditioning.J.Exp.Psychol.Anim.B6(4),339–351,http://dx. doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.6.4.339.

Rescorla,R.A.,1982.EffectofastimulusinterveningbetweenCSandUSin autoshaping.J.Exp.Psychol.Anim.B8(2),131–141,http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ 0097-7403.8.2.131.

Rogers,J.L.,Kesner,R.P.,2006.Lesionsofthedorsalhippocampusorparietalcortex differentiallyaffectspatialinformationprocessing.Behav.Neurosci.120(4), 852–860,http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.120.4.852.

Runyan,J.D.,Moore,A.N.,Dash,P.K.,2004.Aroleforprefrontalcortexinmemory storagefortracefearconditioning.J.Neurosci.24(6),1288–1295,http://dx. doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4880-03.2004.

Weible,A.P.,McEchron,M.D.,Disterhoft,J.F.,2000.Corticalinvolvementin acquisitionandextinctionoftraceeyeblinkconditioning.Behav.Neurosci.114 (6),1058–1067,http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.114.6.1058.

Weible,A.P.,Weiss,C.,Disterhoft,J.F.,2003.Activityprofilesofsingleneuronsin caudalanteriorcingulatecortexduringtraceeyeblinkconditioninginthe rabbit.J.Neurophysiol.90(2),599–612,http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.01097. 2002.

Weiss,C.,Bouwmeester,H.,Power,J.M.,Disterhoft,J.F.,1999.Hippocampallesions preventtraceeyeblinkconditioninginthefreelymovingrat.Behav.BrainRes. 99(2),123–132,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(98)00096-5. Yerkes,R.M.,Dodson,J.D.,1908.Therelationofstrengthofstimulustorapidityof

Figure

Updating...

References

Related subjects : The Trace-Back Procedure