GROWTH
AND
DEVELOPMENT
OF
TWINS
OF
DISSIMILAR
SIZE
AT
BIRTH
S. Gorham Babson, M.D., John Kangas, Ph.D., Norton Young, Ph.D.,
and James L. Bramhall, M.B., Ch.B.
Crippled Children’s Division and Departments of Pediatrics, Medical Psychology, and Clinical Pathology
of the University of Oregon Medical School and Emanuel Hospital, Portland, Oregon
(Submitted July 25; accepted for publication October 15, 1963.)
Presented in part at the 10th International Congress of Pediatrics.
Supported by the Crippled Children’s Division of the University of Oregon Medical School. ADDRESS: (S.G.B.) Department of Pediatrics, University of Oregon Medical School, Portland 1, Oregon.
PEDIATRICS, March 1964
ARTICLES
I
NFANTS undersized for their gestationalage have been reported by many
ob-servers.1-6 Approximately 2% of infants born
at term in the United States of America
weigh 2,500 gm or less and 0.25% of term
infants weigh under 2,050 gm.4
Observa-tions made in our Prematurity Study Clinic
suggested that infants born near term who
were undersized at birth failed to match
the later mental and physical development
of infants of comparable birth weight with
shorter gestation periods.
There is uncertainty among investigators
as to the ultimate outcome of these runted#{176}
but not grossly malformed infants. Some
authors report these infants to be
indistin-guishable from normal babies by 4 to 6
months of age.4’3 Otherse, have
sug-gested that poor fetal growth may be
asso-ciated with failure to achieve maximum
later mental and physical development.
Drillien,b0 in her report on the development
of small premature infants, lists six
ex-amples of twins in five of which the smaller
twin had inferior intelligence at an average
age of 63 years. Bowen and Hepner’12
studying placental abnormalities mention nine runted infants who showed continued
stunting in physical growth through one
*The term, “runt,” is being used in the tra-ditional sense of an unusually small biological product, relative to expectation, and without spe-cification of etiological mechanism(s).
year of age. McCance,13 reviewing animal
experiments states, “The failure of a young
animal to catch up with its fellows or
litter-mates for a long time and indeed its
ten-dency at first to lag further and further
behind after a nutritional setback before
birth is a common experience with many
species.” He further remarks,”... It would
appear that the younger the animal the
more serious a nutritional setback will be,
and permanent effects from under-nutrition
during fetal life are a distinct possibility.”
Twins discrepant in weight at birth
pres-ent an experimental opportunity for
evalu-ating the later effects of intrauterine
growth retardation. The factors of
gesta-tional age, racial background, maternal
health, and nutrition would l)e equated.
Genetic factors would be reduced in the
dizygotic and eliminated in the
mono-zygotic pairs. Differences in psychosocial
environment would also be reduced.
SAMPLE
A search was made for surviving sets of
dissimilar-sized twins in the records of two
Portland, Oregon, hospitals from 1950 to
1958. Only twin sets were selected in which
the runt met the arbitrary criteria of having
been at least 25% smaller than the co-twin
and having weighed less than 2,000 gm at
birth. The records of approximately 40,000
pairs of twins of ‘hom 11 met the criteria.
At the University of Oregon Medical
School Premature Unit the neonatal
rec-ords showed that one or both infants of 87
pairs of twins had been referred. Nine of
these pairs from six different hospitals
ful-filled the study criteria. These admissions
to the Medical School Premature Unit were
believed to have been referred for
eco-nomic reasons, a factor which should not
produce intrapair bias. Nevertheless, the
possibility of selective referral on the basis
of suspected neonatal problems in one twin
cannot be excluded in the referred pairs.
Of the twenty pairs fulfilling the criteria,
eighteen have been traced and sixteen pairs
were available for this study. These
chil-dren
ranged
in age from 4 years, 5 months,to 10 years, 11 months (median of 8%
years) at the time of examination. Fourteen
sets were white, one set Negro, and one set
of Japanese extraction. Eight sets were
female, four sets male, and four sets were
mixed. Nine pairs were monozygotic as
determined by physical similarity and
blood typing.
The average birth weight of the runted
group
was 1.61 kg (range 0.95-1.99 kg) incontrast to the nonrunt group mean of 2.47
kg (range 1.64-3.12 kg). This represents
a mean difference of 0.86
kg
or nearly 2 lb.The runts weighed one-third less on the
average than their co-twin at birth.
Infants of decreasing size may be
ex-pected to have an increasing percentage of
birth weight loss.14 It is of interest that the
average postnatal weight loss in the runted
twins was 0.09 kg (6.6% of their birth
weight)
compared to 0.22kg
(9.2% of theirbirth weight) in the co-twin group. Pick5
reported a similar limitation of weight drop
in a case report
of
three small term infants.The
smaller twin was born first in 9of
the 16 sets. There were 11
cephalic
and
5
breech deliveries in both runted and
non-runted groups. Two runts were male and
2 female in the 4 sets
of unlike
twins. Nomajor differences between groups in
neo-natal complications were found in the
nursery records.
METHOD
Each set of twins was examined by a
pediatrician, clinical psychologist, speech
pathologist, and clinical pathologist.
Infor-mation on the order of births and birth size
was withheld from the examiners.
The examinations included a pediatric
history and physical examination with
an-thropometric measurements. Inspection of
physical characteristics and color
photo-graphs was used for the determination of
zygosity. On the similar appearing twins,
blood typing was carried through nine
anti-sera: anti-A, anti-B, anti-C, anti-D, anti-E,
anti-c, anti-e, anti-M, anti-N, and anti-K
sera. Intelligence was measured by the
Stan-ford-Binet, Form L-M.15
Communication was assessed from both
physical and language aspects. The oral
structures were compared by inspection.
Oral diadochokinesis was timed for distinct
one-, two-, and three-syllable sequences.
Judgments of speech articulation were
made by using phonetic analysis of the
child’s response on a standard articulation
inventory. Hearing was measured by
pure-tone screening audiometry at 15 decibels,
and in the case of the youngest pair, by
speech audiometry. Judgments of language
differences were obtained using
compre-hension items such as association of objects
with
functions and sentence understanding,and communication items such as sentence
length,
complexity,
syntax, and fluency.The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test16
was used to estimate verbal intelligence by
the child’s ability to identify pictures in
response to verbal request.
On quantitative measures, the obtained
paired comparison differences between
runts and their co-twins were evaluated
statistically by testing the mean of a
popula-tion of differences.17 All significance levels
cited refer to one-tailed tests unless noted
otherwise.
RESULTS
Table I presents the raw data derived
from the birth records and the
ri ri ri
C’
03 03 03 03
i-fl
6
00 ‘S “ C’
C’ C’
H
. .5 . . 4
C!)
‘‘i C’ C’ C’ “1 C’ “ C’
C’
‘
-C’. C’ . - C’.
e
, 00
II
,. ‘. ,. 0
,. ,.
Q c n n c. Q c C.) C.) L) C.) C.) 0
.
‘4
.‘
n - n n . , . ,
HO ‘C, :2
C
00 00 00 00 t’.
00 00 00 00 00 00 00 00 03
C
. 0
03 U) U) 0) 0)
rIH
0300 C’ 00 C’ 00 - C’ OC, 00 C’ C’ C’ 09 00 C’
00 ‘0 00 C’ ‘0 00 00 ‘9 00 00 ‘9 ‘9 ‘5
.i
03 03 0) 03 C/: CL
0) 03 CL CL
rf.
03 1.P ‘P - t- 00 00 00 os ‘C, C’ 00 ‘9 -P C’ C’
- ‘p - 00 1.. - C’ 00 C’ CO - CO
- - - -
-11
.
.
0. 03 03
C’ ‘5 C’ C’ C’ ‘0 ‘9 ‘0 C’ ‘9 ‘9 C’ C’
... C’ C’ ‘9 C’ .0 C’ ‘0 C’ ‘0 C’ 05 C’C’ C’ C’ C’
s .. - 9; 9; 9; - ,. t..
‘S
; ‘S-P 00 - C’ C’ C’ C’ ‘p ‘p 9’ 00 03 C’ -P C’ 00 .
- 0 x ‘p e ‘p ‘0 ‘S -‘ C’
00 9’ 09 9’ 9’ - 9’ - 9’ 9’ 9’ 9’ 03 ‘P
- I ?ii
00 00
-C’ .O 00 I
-. .
- 9’ - 9’ - ‘OC - 9’ “C - - 94 - 9’
H II
- 9’ - 09 - 94 94
C.) C.) C.) C.) C.) C.) C.) C.) 0
.
a. a. a. a. a. a. a. a
CJi
. . - . . .
TABLE II
PoYSIcAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN
16 TWIN SIBLING P.snts
TABLE III
INTELLIGENCE DIFFERENCES BETW EEN
16 TWIN SIBLING P.siirs
Test
4tnnford-Binet Form L-M
I’eal)o(ly Picture %ocabular
Mean I.Q. Difference
Runts 96.53 6.73 Nonrunts 103.00
Hunts 91.63 Nonrunts 94.13 . 30
#{149}t p 5.46 <.053 1.73 -.03 Variable Runt Supe-nor 14*
* Sign Test. Walker & I,ev, Statistical Inference,
Henry bit &Co., 1953, 430-431.
t One pair too young to be evaluated.
ARTICLES
331Mean
- .
I ariabie Difference.
avon rig
.\onrunl
I p
Ueight (cm) 4.34 3.86 <.005 Weight (kg) 3.95 5.49 <.000.5
Uead Circumference
(cm) 1.34 4.80 <.0005
Physical differences between the twin
sibling pairs in height, weight, and head
circumference are shown in Table II. The
mean differences on all three variables favor
the nonrunt group to a highly significant
degree. Four children had important
physi-cal defects. They were all runts from
mono-zygotic pairs. The defects were: a patent
ductus arteriosus (corrected), bilateral
zonu-lar cataracts, fusion of the ribs with
scolio-sis, and a monoplegia. The nonrunts had no
comparable deformities.
Shown in Table III are intellectual
dif-ferences between the twin siblings. The
nonrunts as a group show an average
ad-vantage of more than six I.Q. points over
their runted twins on the Stanford-Binet
Intelligence Scale. The mean difference
favoring the nonrunt is statistically
signifi-cant on both of the intelligence measures
used. *
Table IV shows the results of
compari-sons made between runt and nonrunt
groups on speech and language variables.
Judgments of language proved significantly
different between runt and nonrunt siblings
below the 5% level. The other aspects which
relate to production of speech sounds,
speed of articulatory movement, and size
and shape of oral structures were not
sta-* The differences remain statistically significant
when evaluated by the Wilcoxon Matched-Pairs
Signed Ranks Test, a nonparametric statistic which
makes no assumptions concerning the shape of the
distribution of difference scores, S. Siegel:
Non-parametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences,
New York: McGraw-Hill, 1956, pp. 75-83.
tistically different between twin siblings.
Hearing was screened and was normal in
all subjects.
Although the runt and nonrunt groups
are almost evenly divided in their
respec-tive proportions of first- and second-born
children, it is possible to analyze for the
effects of birth order on current height,
weight, head circumference, and
intelli-gence. These differences between first and
second born were not statistically
signifi-cant and are summarized in Table V.
Nine of the 16 sets of twins were judged
to be monozygotic, according to physical
characteristics and identity of the blood
types performed. These 9 pairs offer an
even more precise opportunity to evaluate
the effects of runting, since all genetic
variation would be controlled within each
twin pair. Despite this small subgroup size,
the average differences favoring the
non-flint on the quantitative measures remain
TABLE IV
SPEECH EXAMINATION C0uPARIS0N BET\VEEN
16 TWIN SIBLING P.s1105
Non-Runt !.ren
Supe-nor
Language 3 11 2 .03
Articulation 3 8 5
-Oral Diadochokinesi.sf 7 7 1
-* Based on eight pairs.
TABLE V
PHYsICAL AND INTELLECTUAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN FIR.ST AND SECOND BORN TWIN SIBLINGS
No. Pairs = 16
Variable Mean Difference Favoring Second Born I p* Height (cm) Weight (kg)
Head Circumference (con)
Binet I.Q. 0.84 0.88 0. 2 -0.88 <1 <1 <1 <1
-* A two-tailed test and the .05 significance level used.
statistically significant and of equal or
larger magnitude than in the total group.
Oral diadochokinesis, which previously
showed no trend reaches a statistically
significant level of non-runt superiority in
the monozygotic sub-sample. Table VI
summarizes these comparisons.
CONCLUSIONS
It has been shown that twin pairs can
vary markedly in birth size, and that such
pairs still show significant differences at a
median age of 8% years. These data
con-firm and extend the observations of Bowen
and 12 and Engleson et al.#{176}that
intrauterine growth retardation is associated
with retardation of subsequent growth.
The number of important defects in the
smaller of the twin pairs is consistent with
the data of Warkany et al.’ and Rumboltz
et al. in their reports of an increased
in-cidence of anomalies in children having
had intrauterine growth retardation.
The data also indicate a detrimental
re-lationship between intrauterine runting
and intellectual development. This finding
is in accord with the intellectual differences
apparent in six sets of twins of dissimilar
birth weight listed by Drillien.b0 The work
of Engleson et al. comparing children
dysmature at birth and control children
also found intellectual measures to be
among the discriminating variables.
The language behavior of the nonrunt
group proved to be statistically superior
to the runted group, while factors of speech
sound production, speed of articulation,
and oral structures were not different. It
is of interest to note that runting had a
closer association with the more subtle and
higher order aspects of
communication-those most closely associated with language
and intellect-than with the motor or
pro-duction aspects of speech. The lack of
differences in articulation and motor speech
behavior may be related to the unique
speech environment in a home with twins.
Since Smith’9 has suggested that
intra-uterine stunting may have a genetic
com-ponent, it is of interest to note that the
physical and intellectual disadvantages of
the runt group continued to be exhibited
when only monozygotic pairs are
con-sidered. Birth order was of no significance.
Our data would support the discussion of
Price,20 that natal environment markedly
affects the development of twins. He warns
of interpretations of twin differences based
on post-natal environment, unless
intra-uterine variations have been carefully
eliminated.
SUMMARY
Birth records of a large private hospital and admissions records of a premature re-ferral center, covering the period 1950 to
TABLE VI
PHYSICAL AND INTELLEcTUAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN
9 MONOZYGOTIC TWIN SIBLING PAIRS
1958 were reviewed. Twenty pairs of
sur-viving twins were found in which the
smaller twin weighed less than 2,000 gm
and was at least 25% less than the weight of
the larger twin at birth. Sixteen of these
twenty pairs of twins were examined at a
median age of 8% years.
Comparisons were made between the
group composed of the smaller members
of the twin sets and the group of their
co-twins. A highly significant difference
was present in height, head circumference,
and weight. Significant differences in
in-telligence as measured by the
Stanford-Binet and Peabody Picture Vocabulary
Test, and in the judged level of language
comprehension and expression were found.
No significant difference was found
be-tween the groups in respect to articulation,
oral structures, oral diadochokinesis or
auditory acuity.
Four of the children had important
phys-ical defects. Each child was the smaller
member of a monozygotic twin set.
Birth order showed no relationship to
weight at birth or to subsequent
develop-ment.
A subgroup composed of monozygotic
twins was studied. Results comparable to
those for the total study group were
ob-tained.
REFERENCES
1. Clifford, S. H.: Postmaturity with placental
dysfunction. J. Pediat., 44:1, 1954.
2. Sj#{246}stedt, S., Engleson, G., and Rooth, G.:
Dysmaturity. Arch. Dis. Child., 33:123,
1958.
3. Soderling, B.: Pseudo-prematurity, Acta
Paediat., 42:520, 1953.
4. Rumboltz, W. L., Edwards, M. C., and Mc-Googan, L. S.: The small full term infant
and placental insufficiency. Western J.
Surg., 69:53, 1961.
5. Pick, W.: Malnutrition of the newborn sec-ondary to placental abnormalities. New
Engi. J. Med., 250:905, 1954.
6. Gruenwald, P. : Letter to the Editor: Full
term and low birth weight. PEDIATRICS, 29:
333, 1962.
7. Colman, H. J., and Rienzo, J.: The small full
term baby. Obstet. Gynec., 19:87, 1962.
8. Baird, Sir Dugald: The contribution of
obstet-rica! factors to serious physical and mental
handicap in children. Obstet. Gynaec. Brit.
Emp., 66:749, 1959.
9. Engleson, G., Rooth, C., and T#{246}rnblom, NI.: A
follow-up study of dysmature infants. Arch.
Dis. Child., 38:62, 1963.
10. Drillien, Cecil Mary: The incidence of mental and physical handicaps in school age chil-dren of very low birth weight. PEDIATRICS,
27:452, 1961.
11. Bowen, Mary, and Hepner, R.: Placental
in-sufficiency and growth. J. Dis. Child., 98:
567, 1959.
12. Hepner, R., and Bowen, Mary: The placenta
and the fetus. J.A.M.A., 172:427, 1960.
13. McCance, R. A.: Food, growth, and time.
Lancet, 11:671, 1962.
14. Silverman, W. A.: Dunham’s Premature
In-fants. 3rd Ed., New York: Paul A. Hoeber,
Inc., 1961, pp. 530-538.
15. Terman, L. M., and Merrill, M. N.:
Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale. Boston: Houghton
Mifihin Co., 1960.
16. Dunn, L. M.: Manual for the Peabody Picture
Vocabulary Test. Philadelphia: American
Guidance Service, Inc., 1959.
17. Walker, H., and Lev, J.: Statistical Inference.
New York: Henry Holt, 1953, pp. 151-154.
18. Warkany, J., Monroe, B., and Sutherland,
Betty: Intra-uterine growth retardation.
Amer. J. Dis. Child., 102:127, 1961.
19. Smith, C. A.: Prenatal and neonatal nutrition.
PEDIATRICS, 30:145, 1962.
20. Price, B.: Primary basis in twin studies. A
re-view of perinatal and natal
difference-pro-ducing factors in monozygotic pairs. Amer.
J. Hum. Genet., 2:293, 1950.
Acknowledgment
It is our pleasure to acknowledge the help
pro-vided in this study by Mrs. Sue Underwood, RN.,
Premature Follow-up Nurse; Miss Jeanne Carol
Hopkins, B.S., MT. (ASCP), Research Assistant;
Miss Gwen Amel, RN., Nursery Supervisor of
Emanuel Hospital, and Miss Barbara Goffeney,