• No results found

How Cockpit Design Impacts Pilots’ Attention Distribution and Perceived Workload during Aiming a Stationary Target

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "How Cockpit Design Impacts Pilots’ Attention Distribution and Perceived Workload during Aiming a Stationary Target"

Copied!
7
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

2351-9789 © 2015 The Authors. Published by Elsevier B.V. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

Peer-review under responsibility of AHFE Conference doi: 10.1016/j.promfg.2015.07.781

Procedia Manufacturing 3 ( 2015 ) 5663 – 5669

ScienceDirect

6th International Conference on Applied Human Factors and Ergonomics (AHFE 2015) and the

Affiliated Conferences, AHFE 2015

How Cockpit Design Impacts Pilots’ Attention Distribution and

Perceived Workload during Aiming a Stationary Target

Wen-Chin Li

a

*, Chung-San Yu

b

, Matthew Greaves

a

, Graham Braithwaite

a

a

Safety and Accident Investigation Centre, Cranfield University, Bedfordshire, MK43 0TR, U.K.

b

Department of Industrial Engineering and Engineering Management, National Tsing Hua University, No. 101, Section 2, Kuang-Fu Road, Hsinchu, Taiwan 30013, R.O.C.

Abstract

The eye movement data in five areas of interest (AOIs) were analyzed as follows, Head-up Display (HUD); Integrated Control Panel (ICP); Right Multiple Function Display (RMFD); Left Multiple Function Display (LMFD); and Outside of cockpit (OC). The scenario is performing an air-to-surface task to aim at a stationary target. The results show significant differences in pilots’ percentage of fixation between two interface designs on the ICP (t=-3.36, p<.005, Cohen’s d=-.98); RMFD (t=-4.85, p<.001, Cohen’s d=-1.55) and LMFD (t=-2.56, p<.05, Cohen’s d=-.79). There were significant differences in pilots’ fixation duration between two interfaces on the HUD (t=2.64, p<.05, Cohen’s d=.81); ICP (t=-3.00, p<.005, Cohen’s d=-.94); RMFD (t=-5.32, p<.001, Cohen’s d=-1.65) and LMFD (t=-2.77, p<.05, Cohen’s d=-.92). By the application of eye tracker devices, interface designers can precisely evaluate pilots’ visual behavior among interfaces of cockpit and SA performance. In addition, extra workload might have a negative impact on pilots’ SA performance and increase the probability of operating hazards, and so there is the opportunity to compensate for the negative impact of workload through human-centered design. The current research uses eye-tracking devices to investigate pilots’ visual behaviors and interface design and has potential to facilitate system designers’ understanding of pilots’ attention distribution and situational awareness for improving the integration of cockpit designs and ultimately aviation safety.

© 2015 The Authors. Published by Elsevier B.V. Peer-review under responsibility of AHFE Conference.

Keywords: Attention Distribution; Interface Design; Situation Awareness; Perceived Workload; Visual Scan

* Wen-Chin Li. Tel.: +44-1234-758527;

E-mail address: wenchin.li@cranfield.ac.uk

© 2015 The Authors. Published by Elsevier B.V. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

(2)

1. Introduction

Eye scan pattern is one of the most powerful methods for assessing a pilot’s cognitive processes in the cockpit. The visual information captured by eye trackers provides the opportunity to investigate the relationship between visual scan patterns and attention shifts while performing tasks, as eye tracking devices can provide a vehicle for incorporating considerations of pilot cognitive performance into the design of cockpit displays and interfaces [1&2]. Furthermore, visual behaviors can provide numerous clues concerning the mental process of encoding information perceived by pilots by using in-flight visual behaviors, such as which areas of interest (AOIs) they scan, dwell and attend, as eye movements data are a sensitive and automatic response which may serve as a window into the processing of pilots’ situational awareness (SA) and a reflection of the mental state [3&4]. Endsley [5] defines three levels of SA, which are linked closely with the major components within cognitive processes. The first level is to perceive environmental cues, such as warning lights in the cockpit. The second level is a process of comprehending the cues based on knowledge and experience. The third level is to predict the possible situation in the near future and project the related measurements to resolve the specific status. SA has been recognized an essential component within a pilot’s cognitive process in the domain of aviation [6]. The most important of enhancing pilots’ situation awareness in the flight deck is in the design and implementation of better information displays and human-computer interface. Kaempf and Klein [7] suggested that the quality and difficulty of decisions in aviation are based on the ambiguity and completeness of information. By improving the content and presentation of this information, it is possible to improve and facilitate pilots’ cognitive processes in decision-making. The current research applies eye-tracking devices to investigate pilots’ visual behaviors and interface design. It has potential to facilitate system designers’ understanding of pilots’ cognitive processes relevant to attention distribution and situational awareness, and to influence flight deck interface design.

The natural limitations of human cognitive processes and the vast number of parallel tasks are reasons for increasing critical stress for military pilots. Under the high demands of tactical missions and dynamic aircraft maneuvers, pilots have to deal with additional difficulties and increased workload during multi-missions and adverse hostile conditions such as enemy fire [8]. Because workload can negatively affect operator performance and increase the probability of operating hazards, researchers have put a great deal of effort into developing measures of operator workload [9]. Aside from using self-reported subjective workload ratings as a gauge for evaluating operators’ workload, pupillary response has also been proposed as an index of the amount of cognitive processing [10]. Eye movement measurement offers deep insights into human-machine interaction and the mental processes of pilots. Measurements based on different aspects of ocular behavior, such as the number of fixations, dwell time, and the dilation of pupil, have been used to reveal the status of mental workload. There is evidence that increases in workload could increase both dwell time and the frequency of long fixations [11]. The bottom-up visual process is a stimulus-based and generated from the saliency of information. It can be explained by level one of SA; perception of the cues. On the other side, the top-down approach is a knowledge-based theory centred on the cognitive process [12]. Pilots’ fixation shifts are controlled by cognitive processes to a specific AOI to acquire the information to complete the task in hand. It complies with the theory of three levels of situational awareness proposed by Endsley [13]. A pilot has to perceive the stimulus in the cockpit, understand the encountering situation, and predict the possible consequences in the near future. As visual searching in a flight deck is critical for collecting information, it is the main reason that 75% of pilot errors within cockpit are caused by perceptual failures [14]. The phenomenon highlights the importance of interface design in impacting pilots’ attention distribution, perceived workload, and SA performance [15]. By utilizing a combination of an eye-tracking device and flight simulators, pilots’ eye movement patterns, SA performance and perceived workload could be collected for further analysis, and the results could be served as a feedback loop for improving interface design in the future. Therefore, the objectives of current study are investigating (1) the relationship between interface design and pilots’ SA performance; (2) the relationship between interface design and pilots’ perceived workload; (3) the relationship between pilots’ perceived workload and pilots’ SA performance.

(3)

2. Method

2.1 Subjects

A total of 57 mission-ready military pilots participated in the research. There were 39 Fighter-A type rated pilots and 18 Fighter-B type rated pilots.

2.2 Apparatus

2.2.1. Flight Simulators: Both flight simulators used in the experiment are high-fidelity training devices for military pilots. They are a fixed-base type consisting of identical cockpit displays to those in the military aircraft and are built for the purpose of routine flight training and combat planning. The instructors of both simulators can install scenarios and observe the trainee pilot’s performance via a console with three monitors. The interface designs of Head-up Display (HUD); Integrated Control Panel (ICP); Right Multiple Function Display (RMFD) and Left Multiple Function Display (LMFD) are slightly different in terms of the size and presentation of symbols between Fighter-A and Fighter-B simulators.

The scenario of aiming at a stationary target is known as an air-to-surface (AS) mission. Participants were flying in a patrol area at an attitude of 20,000 feet, heading of 050° with a cruise speed of 300 knots indicated air speed (KIAS) under visual flight rules (VFR). While pilots were dispatched to attack one surface stationary target, they not only needed to execute tasks precisely by operating the aircraft, but also to follow the navigation system, entering appropriate codes by using various flight deck interfaces. Participants had to intercept the correct route and turn toward the target at an altitude of 500 feet with a speed of 500 KIAS simultaneously. They then performed a steep pop-up manoeuver to increase altitude abruptly for appropriate target reconnaissance, followed by a dive and roll-in toward the surface target to avoid hostile radar lock-on. When approaching the target, participants have to roll-out, level the aircraft, aim at the target, lock-on and pick-off the weapons.

2.2.2 Eye Tracking Device: Pilot’s eye movement patterns were recorded by using a mobile head-mounted eye tracker (ASL Series 4000), which is designed by Applied Science Laboratory. It is a light (76 g) and portable device which enables subjects to move their head without any limitations during the air-to-air maneuvers. Pilots’ eye movements and the related data were collected and stored in a Digital Video Cassette Recorder (DVCR) and then transferred to a computer for further analysis. The definition of a fixation point was three gaze points occurring within an area of 10 by 10 pixels with a 200 millisecond dwell time (the time spent per glance at an area or display of instrument). The eye movement data in five AOIs were defined as following, AOI-1: HUD; AOI-2: ICP; AOI-3: RMFD; AOI-4: LMFD; and AOI-5: Outside of cockpit. These AOIs provided critical information for pilots performing the task of air-to-air maneuvers.

2.2.3 NASA-TLX: The NASA Task Load Index (TLX) is a popular technique for measuring subjective mental workload. It relies on a multidimensional construct to derive an overall workload score based on a weighted average of ratings on six subscales: (1) Mental demand: How much mental demand and perceptual activities you would use; (2) Physical demand: How much is the degree of physical demand? (3) Temporal demand: How much is the degree of time pressure? (4) Performance: How do you feel about the flying time and the performance in flight? (5) Effort: How difficult do you think the task is? (6) Frustration: How much frustration and disappointment do you feel? [16].

2.3 Research Design

All of the Fighter-A and Fighter-B subject pilots undertook the following procedures, (1) completed the demographical data including training experience and total flight hours (5 minutes); (2) a briefing of the study and the air-to-surface scenario (10 minutes); (3) calibration of the eye tracking device by using three points distributed over the cockpit interface display and screen (10-15 minutes); (4) participants performed the air-to-surface task, simultaneously instructor evaluated participants’ situational awareness by activating a warning light during pursuit the target (3-5 minutes); (5) participants evaluate perceived workload by NASA-TLX (5-10 minutes); (6) debriefing for participants’ feedback (10-15 minutes).

(4)

3. Results

There were fifty-seven mission-ready fighter pilots (39 Fighter-A pilots; 18 Fighter-B pilots) who participated in this research. The ages of Fighter-A pilots are between 26 and 45 years old (M=33, SD=5); total flying hours between 372 and 3,200 hours (M=1304, SD=811); type flying hours between 89 and 2,150 hours (M=809, SD=558). The ages of Fighter-B pilots are between 26 and 39 years old (M=29, SD=3); total flying hours between 310 and 1,600 hours (M=778, SD=344); type flying hours between 100 and 1,200 hours (M=473, SD=307). The participants’ demographic data are shown in table 1. Subjects’ percentage of fixation and average fixation duration are shown as table 2; participants’ perceived workload by NASA-TLX is shown as table 3. Furthermore, the performance of situational awareness between Fighter-A and Fighter-B pilots is present as table 4.

Table 1. Participants’ demographic variables.

Variables Groups Frequencies

Age 25-30 29 (50.9%) 31-35 13 (22.8%) 36-40 8 (14%) 41-45 7 (12.3%) Rank Lieutenant 1 (1.8%) Captain 32 (56.1%) Major 10 (17.5%) Lieutenant Colonel 13 (22.8%) Colonel Above 1 (1.8%) Qualification Combat ready 25 (43.9%)

Two fighter team leader 8 (14%) Four fighter team leader 12 (21%) Daytime back seat instructor 3 (5.3%) Training instructor 9 (15.8%)

Total Flight Hours

500 and less 9 (15.8%)

501-1000 22(38.6%)

1001-1500 14 (24.6%)

1501-2000 5 (8.8%)

2001 and above 7 (12.2%)

The ‘percentage of fixation’ is proportional data, and it has to be transformed as an arcsine value before conducting analysis of variance (Table 2). There were significant differences in pilots’ percentage of fixation between two different designs of Fighter-A and Fighter-B on the ICP 3.36, p<.005, Cohen’s d=-.98); RMFD (t=-4.85, p<.001, Cohen’s d=-1.55) and LMFD (t=-2.56, p<.05, Cohen’s d=-.79). Moreover, there were significant differences in pilots’ average fixation duration between two different designs of Fighter-A and Fighter-B on the HUD (t=2.64, p<.05, Cohen’s d=.81); ICP (t=-3.00, p<.005, Cohen’s .94); RMFD (t=-5.32, p<.001, Cohen’s d=-1.65) and LMFD (t=-2.77, p<.05, Cohen’s d=-.92).

Table 2. Percentage of fixation and average fixation duration among AOIs between Fighter-A and Fighter-B pilots.

Variables AOIs Fighter Types T-Test A B M SD M SD t df p SE Cohen's d Percentage of fixation (arcsine) HUD 54.49 11 55.15 9.06 -0.22 55 .825 2.97 -0.07 ICP 4.19 4.58 8.42 4.02 -3.36 55 .001 1.26 -0.98 RMFD 1.2 2.63 8.5 6.14 -4.85 19.94 .000 1.51 -1.55 LMFD 0.35 1.52 2.05 2.63 -2.56 22.44 .018 0.67 -0.79 OC 34.5 10.19 31.02 8.63 1.25 55 .215 2.77 0.37

Average fixation duration (msec) HUD 541 108 468 69 2.64 55 .011 28 0.81 ICP 170 211 330 118 -3.00 52.84 .001 44 -0.94 RMFD 60 138 386 243 -5.32 22.24 .000 61 -1.65 LMFD 14 60 212 300 -2.77 17.62 .013 71 -0.92 OC 390 82 425 64 -1.60 55 .114 22 -0.48

(5)

Table 3 indicates the results of pilots' perceived workload assessed by the NASA-TLX. There were significant differences in physical demand (t=3.42, p<.005, Cohen’s d=.87); effort (t=4.05, p<.001, Cohen’s d=1.04) and total workload (t=3.02, p<.005, Cohen’s d=.88) between Fighter-A and Fighter-B pilots. Furthermore, there was approaching significance in mental demand (t=-1.82, p=.074, Cohen’s d=-.52) The Fighter-A pilots (M=61.09, SD=10.55) show significantly higher workload than Fighter-B pilots (M=52.34, SD=9.25). In addition, table 4 indicates that 92.3 % of Fighter-A pilots show significantly good situational awareness as evidenced through the identification of a warning signal compared with 55.6% of Fighter-B pilots.

Table 3. Perceived workload by NSAS-TLX between Fighter-A and Fighter-B pilots. Variables Fighter Types T-Test A B M SD M SD t df p SE Cohen's d Mental Demand 11.78 6.61 15.25 6.85 -1.82 55 .074 1.91 -0.52 Physical Demand 9.68 8.8 3.44 4.95 3.42 52.65 .001 1.83 0.87 Temporal Demand 10.67 9.03 6.91 5.67 1.62 55 .111 2.32 0.50 Performance 12.55 7.64 16.35 7.68 -1.75 55 .087 2.18 -0.50 Effort 10.19 6.12 5 3.5 4.05 52.26 .000 1.28 1.04 Frustration 6.22 5.78 5.39 4.78 0.53 55 .597 1.56 0.16 Total workload 61.09 10.55 52.34 9.25 3.02 55 .004 2.90 0.88

Table 4. Situational awareness performance between Fighter-A and Fighter-B pilots.

ġ SA Performance Total

Non-identified warning signal Identified warning signal

Fighter A 3 36 39 (7.7%) (92.3%) Fighter B 8 10 18 (44.4%) (55.6%) Total 11 46 57 4. Discussion

The concept of human-centered flight deck design is to ensure that pilots are familiar with the cockpit layout and to provide pilots with general techniques for dealing with unexpected and possibly poorly defined problems. There are several approaches which might contribute to enhancing pilots’ situational awareness in the cockpit, such as training, systems integration, human-centered design, and interventions aimed at dealing with workload effects. Dekker [17] proposes that human errors are systematically connected to features of people's tools and tasks. Understanding why people do what they do so we can tweak, change the world in which they work and shape their assessments and actions accordingly is an important aim. The current research find that pilots distributed a high percentage of fixation on the HUD (AOI-1) and outside of the cockpit (AOI-5). It also showed that pilots paid the majority of attention to acquiring in-flight information mainly from these two AOIs, no matter which type of cockpit interface they operated (table 2). Furthermore, table 2 also indicates pilots’ percentage of fixation and average fixation duration on ICP, RMFD and LMFD had significant differences between two types of cockpit design. Moreover, Fighter-B pilots have higher percentage of fixation and longer fixation duration than Fighter-A pilots among ICP, RMFD and LMFD. This phenomenon might indicate Fighter-B pilots have to allocate more attention among those AOIs to complete the tasks. On the other hand, the finding probably showed that Fighter-A pilots can complete the same task mostly according to the information provided only by the HUD. This assumption can also be supported by the longest fixation duration on the HUD for Fighter-A pilots (541 msec) compared to Fighter-B pilots (468 msec), for the length of fixation duration is the total time fixating on an AOI, which reflects the level of importance of information [18]. In addition, the phenomenon that Fighter-A pilots having longer fixation duration on the HUD is probably due to more complex and important information needed to be processed. The findings of current research support previous studies [1, 3] that in-flight visual behaviors recorded by eye-tracking devices could be beneficial to aviation professionals and cockpit designers in understanding of pilots’ cognitive processes

(6)

and attention distributions. Pilots’ attention shifts are the foundation of gaining situational awareness, and pilots have to make decisions based on the interpreting information presented through the interface which in turn reflect the system designers’ original concepts [19].

Operating multi-tasks under time pressure might create huge workload for military pilots. Workload is one of critical factors impacting pilots’ situational awareness [15]. Table 3 indicates that Fighter-A pilots’ perceived workload (61.09) by NSAS-TLX is higher than Fighter-B pilots (52.34). The results showed that Fighter-A pilots’ perceived Physical Demand, Temporal Demand and Effort are significant higher than Fighter-B pilots. However, Fighter-A pilots’ situational awareness (92.3%) was significantly higher than Fighter-B pilots (55.6%) in terms of identifying a warning signal during tactical maneuvers (table 4). This phenomenon seems to be incompatible with the previous finding that workload could result in ineffective attention management and inadequate problem detection [20]. However, the results of current research might demonstrate that the design of the warning signal, which appeared at the HUD of Fighter-A, very much aided pilots’ situational awareness, which can compensate for the impact of workload. Alternatively, both Fighter-A and Fighter-B pilots were under the highest workload situations of Air-to-Surface mission, and the more human-centered the design, the better SA performance. However, the complexity of information presented on the HUD of Fighter-A should be appreciated by system designers, as it is likely to have potential to distract pilots’ from their visual scans, especially during dynamic tactical maneuvers.

If human factors specialists can be more aware of design-inducing human errors early in the design process, they can design the best-fit interfaces and optimize pilots’ performance, improve aviation safety, and reduce costs. Understanding pilots’ in-flight visual behavior is one of the necessary methods to engineers in designing the human-centered interfaces [21]. Pilots’ visual acuity is restricted to only a small area around the fixation point and the control of eye movements is essential for good performance of situational awareness [22]. Jensen [23] proposed that there are two major objectives for human factors; the primary objective of human factors research is to design systems in the most optimum way to take advantage of the characteristics and abilities of the people who are expected to operate them; the second objective is to select and train the operators of the systems. Furthermore, by applying the concept of human-centered design and system integration, designers can minimize the need for training. In other words, training can become and means to complete the job left undone by the systems design. A consideration of enhanced situation awareness in the cockpit provide a means of design for task-oriented behavior in dynamic systems, and it shifts the design process from providing operators with data to providing information for pilots performing tactical tasks. Another benefit of human-centered design is that it facilitates training in realistic conditions, creating a richer level of information to validate the designed products. By the application of real-time eye tracker devices, interface designers can precisely evaluate pilots’ visual behavior among interfaces of cockpit and SA performance. In addition, extra workload might have a negative impact on pilots’ SA performance and increase the probability of operating hazards, and so there is the opportunity to compensate for the negative impact of workload through human-centered design. The current research uses eye-tracking devices to investigate pilots’ visual behaviors and interface design and has potential to facilitate system designers’ understanding of pilots’ attention distribution and situational awareness for improving the integration of cockpit designs and ultimately aviation safety.

References

[1] U. Ahlstrom, F.J. Friedman-Berg, Using Eye Movement Activity as a Correlate of Cognitive Workload, Int. J. Ind. Ergonom., 36 (2006): 623–636.

[2] H. Ayaz, B. Willems, B. Bunce, P.A. Shewokis, K. Izzetoglu, S. Hah, B. Onaral, in: T. Marek, W. Karwowski, V. Rice (Eds.), Advances in Understanding Human Performance: Neuroergonomics, Human Factors Design, and Special Populations, CRC Press, Taylor & Francis Group, 2010, pp. 21–32.

[3] D.D. Salvucci, J.R. Anderson, Tracing Eye Movement Protocols with Cognitive Process Models, in: Proceedings of the Twentieth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, New Jersey, 1998, pp. 923–928.

[4] F.Y. Kuo, C.W. Hsu, R.F. Day, An Exploratory Study of Cognitive Effort Involved in Decision under Framing-An Application If the Eye-Tracking Technology, Decis. Support Syst., 48(2009): 81–91.

[5] M.R. Endsley, Toward a Theory of Situation Awareness in Dynamic Systems, Hum. Factors, 37(1995): 32–64.

[6] Y.W. Sohn, S.M. Doane, Memory Processes of Flight Situation Awareness: Interactive Roles of Working Memory Capacity, Long-Term Working Memory, and Expertise, Hum. Factors, 46(2004): 461–475.

(7)

[7] G.L. Kaempf, G. Klein, Aeronautical Decision Making: The Next Generation. in: N. Johnston, N. McDonald, R. Fuller (Eds.), Aviation Psychology in Practice, Ashgate, England, 1994, pp. 223–254.

[8] U. Ahlstrom, Current Trends in the Display of Aviation Weather, J. Air Traf. Cont, 45 (2003): 14–21.

[9] P. Averty, C. Collet, A. Dittmar, S. Athe´nes, E. Vernet-Maury,. Mental Workload in Air Traffic Control: An Index Constructed from Feld Tests, Aviat. Space Envir. Md, 75(2004): 333–341.

[10] J. Beatty, Task-evoked Pupillary Responses, Processing Load, and the Structure of Processing Resources, Psychol. Bull, 91(1982): 276–292. [11] K. F. Van Orden, W. Limbert, S. Makeig, T. P. Jung, Eye Activity Correlates of Workload during a Visuospatial Memory Task. Hum.

Factors, 43(2001): 111–121.

[12] J.M. Henderson, Human Gaze Control during Real-World Scene Perception, Trends Cogn. Sci., 7(2003): 498–504.

[13] M.R. Endsley, in: C.E. Zsambok, G. Klein (Eds.), Naturalistic Decision Making, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, 1997, pp. 269– 284.

[14] D.G. Jones, M.R. Endsley, Sources of Situation Awareness Error in Aviation, Aviat. Space Envir. Md., 67(1996):507–512.

[15] M.R. Endsley, B. Bolte, D.G. Jones, Designing for Situation Awareness: An Approach to User-Centered Design, Taylor & Francis, New York, 2003.

[16] S.G. Hart, L.E. Staveland, in: P.A. Hancock, N. Meshkati (Eds.), Human Mental Workload, North Holland Press, Amsterdam, 1988. [17] S. Dekker, Follow the Procedure or Survive. Hum. Factors Aero. Safety, 1(2001): 381-385.

[18] F.T. Durso, A. Sethumadhavan, Situation Awareness: Understanding Dynamic Environments, Hum. Factors, 53(2008): 442–448.

[19] J. Orasanu, Crew Collaboration in Space: A Naturalistic Decision-Making Perspective. Aviat. Space Envir. Md., 76(2005, Suppl.):B154–163. [20] R. Lipshitz, G. Klein, J. Orasanu, E. Salas, Focus Article: Taking Stock of Naturalistic Decision Making, J. Behav. Decis. Making, 14(2001):

331–352.

[21] T. Dukic, L. Hanson, K. Holmqvist, C. Wartenberge, Effect of Button Location on Driver's Visual Behaviour and Safety Perception, ERGONOMICS, 48(2005): 399–410.

[22] A. Johnson, R.W. Proctor, Attention: Theory and Practice, Sage Publications, Inc., London, 2004.

[23] R.S. Jensen, The Boundaries of Aviation Psychology, Human Factors, Aeronautical Decision Making, Situation Awareness, and Crew Resource Management, Int. J. Aviat. Psychol, 7(1997): 259–268.

References

Related documents

Our analysis combines data from multiple sources: longitudinal household sur- vey data gathered in 2002 and 2004, administrative data on contributions and fund choices from

// For North Charlotte Waterworks, Inc.. F ind attached the Affidavit of Publication of the New spaper publication.. N orth Charlotte Waterworks, Inc. 1 s not request1ng a

A map unit dominated by a soil with a very dark mineral surface horizon, 30 cm thick, with high base saturation, no secondary carbonates, clay illuviation features and

1 Reported combined ratio of 100.7% adjusted for technical Corona effects in non-life business: EUR 102m premiums impact, EUR 565m claims (net) not absorbed by otherwise unused

The dependent variables in the present study were (a) measures of child behaviors in their home settings as reported by parents via daily reports (Chamberlain &amp; Reid, 1987),

Leave highway E45 at exit number 42 Continue at Hadstenvej for 0,6 kilometres Continue at Århusvej for 3,2 kilometres Continue at Randers Bro 0,3 kilometres.. Turn to the

Using this question- naire, it was to be found out what service foci the respective enterprise has, in which economic sectors (fields of activity) workers are provided and

Current research endeavors in Li-ion battery for EV application are similar to those of NiMH: new high capacity metal oxide cathode, high capacity anode, and new electrolyte with