• No results found

PSYCHOLOGICAL SEQUELAE TO BACTERIAL MENINGITIS: TWO CONTROLLED STUDIES

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2020

Share "PSYCHOLOGICAL SEQUELAE TO BACTERIAL MENINGITIS: TWO CONTROLLED STUDIES"

Copied!
8
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

(

Received April 5; revision accepted for publication July 19, 1971.)

Presented, in part, to the American Pediatric Society-Society for Pediatric Research meeting, May 2, 1970.

Presented, in part, at the meeting of the XIII International Congress of Pediatrics in Vienna,

Sep-tember 1, 1971.

This study was supported by grants from the U.S. Public Health Service A1-06110, NIH Vanderbilt

University General Research Grant #96, in part by the U.S. Office of Education Grant OEG 0 8

070644-1753.

ADDRESS FOR REPRINTS: (S.H.W.S.) Department of Pediatrics, Vanderbilt University School of

Medi-cine, Nashville, Tennessee 37203.

PELwrnxcs, Vol. 49, No. 2, February 1972

PSYCHOLOGICAL

SEQUELAE

TO

BACTERIAL

MENINGITIS:

TWO

CONTROLLED

STUDIES

Sarah H. W. Sell, M.D., Warren

W. Webb,

Ph.D.,

John E. Pate, Ed.D.,

and Emanuel 0. Doyne,

M.D.

From the Departments of Pediatrics and Psychiatry, Vanderbilt University School of Medicine,

Nashville, Tennessee

ABSTRACT. Two controlled studies concern the

results of standardized psychological test assess-ment of long-term sequelae of bacterial meningitis

inschool age children who were ill before age 3. In the first group, each survivor was matched

with a near-age, nonmeningitic sibling. Results of

the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children indi-cated that the mean IQ of the post-meningitic chil-dren was 86; control children, 97 (p = <.05).

Comparison of each subject with his control

re-vealed that six (29%) were 15 IQ points (1

stan-dard deviation) below the control. In two (10%)

the difference exceeded 30 IQ points (2 standard deviations). There were six subjects with IQ scores higher than controls but none by 15 points.

The second investigation compared survivors

who were considered to be entirely normal with

matched peers in the same classroom. Results of

the dependent measures-Illinois Test of Psycholin-guistic Abilities, the Frostig Developmental Test of Visual Perception, and the Peabody Picture Vocab-ulary Test-all indicated that the post-meningitic children functioned at significantly lower levels

than their nonmeningitic peers.

It was concluded that both groups of survivors

were damaged by the disease. The evidence

sup-ports the need for prevention rather than reliance upon cure of meningitis. Pediatrics, 49:212, 1972, MENINGITIS, SEQUELAE, PSYCHOLOGICAL TEST

ASSESS-MENT.

T

HE dramatic reduction of mortality from meningitis due to Hemophilus in-fluenzae, Neiseria meningitic, and

Diplo-coccus penumoniae, by the use of

antibiot-ics, is one of the great achievements of the

past two decades. However, now that

bac-terial antigens have been developed for possible prevention of serious infections due to these microorganisms,15 there is need for data concerning the results of the long-term effects of meningitis in patients who

survived under the present regime of

treat-ment. Such information would be useful in

evaluating the need for prevention by

im-munoprophylaxis.

Two recent reports concerned the

long-term, overall functioning of survivors of H. influenzae meningitis.6’7 Both studies found that less than half of the children escaped detectable sequelae. However, there were no control subjects for either study, which was considered a weakness by both groups

of authors.

This report presents the results of

stan-dardized psychological test assessment of long-term sequelae of bacterial meningitis

in two controlled studies. The first investi-gation concerns the Wechsler Intelligence

(2)

TABLE I AGES OF SIBLING PAIRS AT TESTING

TABLE II

ARTICLES

were considered to be entirely normal with matched peers in the same classroom. De-pendent measures included the Illinois Test

of Psycholinguistic Abilities, the Frostig Developmental Test of Visual Perception, and the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test.

FIRST STUDY

Selection of Subjects

From the records of the Pediatric Bacte-riologic Laboratory of the Vanderbilt Uni-versity Medical Center, patients were

iden-tified who had cerebrospinal fluid or blood cultures from which Hemophilus influenzae

was grown during the period June 1960,

through June 1964. Information from their clinical charts determined survivors of

pu-rulent meningitis who were 2 months to 3

years of age at the time of onset of the ill-ness. All such patients who had a near-age

sibling to act as control were invited to

re-turn to Vanderbilt University Medical Center during the summer of 1969 for a

se-ries of tests. Travel costs were paid for those who indicated need. When there was a choice, the sibling of same sex and nearest

age was selected as control. Twenty-one

pairs of children accepted the invitation. All

except two subjects returned to Vanderbilt

for the evaluation. One severely retarded

child was tested at his special residential fa-cility. One nearest age, same sex sibling

control was congenitally deaf, and was tested at her special school.

PSYCHOLOGICAL TESTING: Each child was

appraised by a skilled psychological

tech-nician who did not know which was con-trol (C) or post-meningitic (PM). The

Age at Testing Sex

Test Groups

Mean Sid.

Des. M F

Post-meningitic (PM) (N=1) 8.4 ±1.5 13 8

Controls (C) (N=1) 10.8 ±.5 1 9

Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC)8 was selected for the evaluation of intellectual functioning since it covers a

variety of tasks with 10 subtests each of

which is standardized for age. The I.Q.

scores allow direct comparisons of children at different ages and can be related to some of the other standardized tests such as Stan-ford-Binet.9

STATISTICAL METHODS: The data were

analyzed by the t-test and by the sign test.1#{176}

Results

Table I presents the composition of the subject population. Males predominated in both groups.

Table II presents the basic results from

this study. It will be noted that the mean I.Q. score for the PM subjects was 86 while

C was 97, a significant difference.

Figure 1 is a graphic representation of I.Q. results of each subject with his control.

It shows that there was a wide variation in

the I.Q. scores of the nonmeningitic controls,

ranging from 148 to 62, with nine below 90 and four below 80. There were 14 PM chil-dren who were functioning below their

INTELLIGENCE TmT RIBTJLTS

N Mean I.Q.

test Wilcoxson’s Si gn Test

t p Lower Rank Total p

Post-Meningitic (P.M.) 1 86

.16 .04 5.5 <.05

(3)

#{149}posi MENINGITIC OCONTROL

214

Ui -J U Cl)

a

Fic. 1. Representation of I.Q. results of each subject with his control (21 sibling pairs).

controls. The difference for six pairs

(

29%)

was 15 I.Q. points

(

1 standard deviation) below while in two pairs

(

10%) the differ-ence exceeded 30 I.Q. points (2 standard deviations). There were six PM subjects who had I.Q. scores higher than controls but none by 15 points.

No statistically significant correlation was

found between the difference in I.Q.’s and the patients’ length of hospitalization or age at onset of illness or difference in age from his sibling at the time of the testing.

SECOND STUDY

Selection of Subjects

From the records of hospitals and prac-ticing pediatricians in Nashville, Tennessee,

25 PM subjects were identified who had

pu-rulent meningitis, 15 due to H. influenzae,

five to meningococcus, and five to pneu-monococcus. Each child received adequate

therapy for the illness, survived, and ap-peared to be free from sequelae. Of those

who were considered to be normal at school age, the 25 were selected from enrollees in

the first three grades in regular public

school classes of the Nashville Metropolitan

School System in 1969. Those who had ob-vious sequelae, or who were

institutional-ized, or who required special educational

services were excluded from the sample.

Each child was matched by age, sex, social

class,h1 and classroom membership with a

nonmeningitic peer. To select the controls, all students in the classroom of the same sex

as an index subject were given a personal

(4)

TABLE III AGES OF PEER PAIRS AT TESTING

ARTICLES

One child was selected at random from those that matched. The parents were inter-viewed to obtain written informed consent

for participation in the study.

PSYcHOLOGICAL TESTING: Although the

pairs of subjects were assessed along a wide

spectrum of parameters, of primary import

for this report were three standardized psychological tests:

(

1

)

The Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abffities,IS which is di-vided into 10 subtests, evaluates the ability

of the subject to receive, integrate and

ex-press language. A raw score and a

psy-cholinguistic age can be calculated from the results. (2) The Frostig Developmental Test of Visual Perception,13 evaluates five facets of visual perceptual development, and the results are expressed as a perceptual

quotient. (3) The Peabody Picture

Vocab-ulary Test,14 is a relatively simple means of

testing word knowledge. At the sound of a

test word, the subject merely points to one of four pictures, the one he believes is most appropriate. The result is expressed as a vocabulary quotient.

PROCEDURE: The children were

trans-ported in pairs from their school to the Vanderbilt University Medical Center for

evaluation. A skilled psychological

techni-cian administered the tests without knowl-edge of which individual was survivor or

control.

Results

Table III represents the subject

popula-tion, matched for age and sex. With each of the tests for instructional receptivity the survivor subjects had lower mean scores than the peer controls.

Table IV shows the results of the Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities. The mean psycholinguistic raw score of the sur-vivors was 225.48 while controls scored 327.00. This difference is significant at the

.004 level (t-test two tailed).

Table V indicates results of the Frostig Test. The mean perceptual quotient of the

survivor subjects was

92.36

while that for controls was 101.84; the probability for this

Age at Testing Sex

Test Groups

-Mean S.D. Al F

Post-meningitic (PM) (N = 5) 7. 1 ± .9 16 9

Controls (C) (N=25) 7.8 ±1.0 16 9

difference in means was .084 which is not

quite significant, (t-test, two tailed). How-ever, the mean difference for the subject,

position in space, was signfficant

(

t = 2.19,

p = .031).

Table VI presents the results for the

Pea-body Picture Vocabulary Test. The mean

vocabulary quotient of the survivors was

90.96 while that for controls was 102.60. This difference is significant at the .035

level (t-test, two tailed).

DISCUSSION

The sibling controls, as a whole, had a wide range of I.Q. scores. The differences

between the pairs gave indication of what

the PM’s might have been. Both groups of

survivors of bacterial meningitis, despite

acceptable therapy received during the

ill-ness at early ages, were functioning

intel-lectually below their controls. In the second study the children were specifically selected because they were thought to be entirely undamaged by their experience with

men-ingitis. That these were having learning

TABLE IV

ILLINOIS TEST OF PSYCHOLINGUISTIC ABILITIES*t

Al ean Psycho-linguistic Raw Score Standard Deviation t-te st -p

Post-meningitic 5 .48 116.89 3.055 .004

(N=5)

Control (N=5) 37.00 118.05

(5)

* Both distributions are normally distributed. t There is homogeneity of variance.

TABLE V

FROSTIG DEVELOPMENTAL TEST OF

VISUAL PERcEp’nout

Mean 1-test

Standard ____________

Perceptual

Deviation -__________

Quotient t p

Post-meningitic

(N=5) 9.36

1.74 .084

Control (N=5) 101.84 15.70

* Both distributions are normally distributed.

t There is heterogeneity of variance, but with ap-propriate formula, t is still 1.74.

problems which were undetected

prey-ously is especially significant.

The selection of controls for children is

always controversial. It can be argued that

siblings have their individual differences. Matched peers certainly differ in many

ways. But the important fact is that the

ex-perimental results with both classes of

con-trols led to the same general conclusion that survivors were damaged by their illness.

The weight of evidence points to the need for prevention rather than reliance upon cure of the disease.

The differential influences of child-rear-ing practices and the differential reactions

of families to serious illness in their

chil-dren were not examined in this report, but these factors should be of substantial conse-quence. For example, it is interesting to speculate that within families, social

pres-TABLE VI

PEABODY PICTURE VOCABULARY TERT*t

Mean Vocabulary Quotient Standard Deviation t-test I p

Post-meningitic 90.96 Qi .24

(N=25) 2.139 .035

Control

(N=25) 102.60 17.00

sures on the relatively slow child

(

even though his I.Q. be above 90) but whose

sib-lings were very bright, might be greater than that experienced by a child with a

be-low

average

I.Q. whose family members were also functioning in the below average range.

SUMMARY

Two groups

of survivors

of proven

bacte-rial meningitis before age 3 years were

tested for intellectual functioning after school age. In the first group, each survivor was matched with a near-age sibling. Re-suits of the Wechsier Intelligence Scale for Children indicated that the mean I.Q. of these children was significantly lower than that of the controls. The second group

con-sisted of survivors considered to be entirely free of sequelae who were enrolled in the

first three grades in public school classes.

Each was matched by age, sex, social class, and classroom membership with a

nonmen-ingitic peer. Results of tests for instructional

receptivity indicated that the survivors

functioned at significantly lower levels than

their nonmeningitic peers. It was

con-cluded that both groups of survivors of bacterial meningitis were damaged by the disease. The evidence supports the need for prevention rather than reliance upon cure.

REFERENCES

1. Austrian, R., and Gold, J.: PDeUInOCOCCaI

bac-teremia with especial reference to bacter-ernie Pneumococcal pneumonia. Ann. Intern.

Med., 60:759, 1964.

2. Artenstein, M. S., Gold, R., Zimmerly, J. C.,

Wyle, C., Schneider, H., and Harkins, C.:

Prevention of meningococcal disease by

Group C polysaccharide vaccine. New Eng. J. Med., 282:417, 1970.

3. Rodrigues, L. P., and Robbins, J. B.: The

iso-lation and immunogenic properties of the

capsular polysaccharide of Hemophilus influ-enzae, type b. (Abst. ) Pediat. Res., 3:361, 1969.

4. Robbins, J. B., Schneerson, R., Rodrigues, L. P.,

(6)

type b. (Abst.) Pediat. Res., 4:376, 1970. 5. Smith, D. H., Anderson, P., Johnston, R. B., Jr.,

Berkel, I. A., and Pitt, J.: The role of polyri-bosephosphate in immunity to Hemophilus influenzae b. (Abst.) J. Clin. Invest., 49:91,

1970.

6. Sell, S. H. W., Merrill, R. E., Doyne, E. 0., and Zimsky, E. P.: Long term sequelae of Hemo-philus infiuenzae meningitis. Pwrmcs, 49:206, 1972.

7. Sproles, E. T., III, Azarrad, J., Williamson, C.,

and Merrill, R. E.: Meningitis due to

Hemo-philus influenzae: Long term sequelae. J.

Pediat., 75:782, 1969.

8. Wechsler, D.: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for

Children (WISC), New York: The

Psycho-logical Corporation, 1949.

9. Pinneau, S. R.: Changes in Intelligence

Quo-tient. Boston: Houghton Muffin Co., p. 106, 1961.

10. Bruning, J. L., and Kintz, B. L.:

Computa-tional Handbook for Statistics. Glenview, Ii-linois: Scott-Foresman and Co., 1960.

11. Hollingshead, A. B.: Two Factor Index of

So-cial Position. New Haven, Connecticut: Pri-vate Memo., 1957.

12. McCarthy, J. J., and Kirk, S. A.: Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities. Urbana, Illi-nois: Institute for Research on Exceptional

Children, 1961.

13. Frostig, M.: Developmental Test of Visual Per-ception. Palo Alto, California: Consulting Psychologists Press, 1963.

(7)

1972;49;212

Pediatrics

Sarah H. W. Sell, Warren W. Webb, John E. Pate and Emanuel O. Doyne

CONTROLLED STUDIES

PSYCHOLOGICAL SEQUELAE TO BACTERIAL MENINGITIS: TWO

Services

Updated Information &

http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/49/2/212

including high resolution figures, can be found at:

Permissions & Licensing

http://www.aappublications.org/site/misc/Permissions.xhtml

entirety can be found online at:

Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures, tables) or in its

Reprints

http://www.aappublications.org/site/misc/reprints.xhtml

(8)

1972;49;212

Pediatrics

Sarah H. W. Sell, Warren W. Webb, John E. Pate and Emanuel O. Doyne

CONTROLLED STUDIES

PSYCHOLOGICAL SEQUELAE TO BACTERIAL MENINGITIS: TWO

http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/49/2/212

the World Wide Web at:

The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is located on

American Academy of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 1073-0397.

References

Related documents

Thus, main objective of this study was to analyze the potential genetic diversity among genotypes of chilli and to classify the genotypes into different groups based

Likewise, if you put your pencil mark to the left or the right of your cutting point, you will need to make your cut to the left or right of the mark.. There’s no real right or

We know remarkably little about how police actually question delinquents. Most of what the legal community-judges, law professors and criminologists, and

In March 2001, the Ministry of Education officials from the Department of Special Needs collaborated with professionals from the Universiti Putra Malaysia (UPM) to formulate a

A magnet based interior nodes insertion method and Delaunay based Triangulation are used to produce uniform garment mesh.. The back and front garment meshes are

The Dutch accountability system includes both measures of cognitive outcomes (primarily student achievement results on the so-called Cito test in Grade 8) and school inspections

Figure 2 shows the IRAM 30 m 239 − 243 GHz spectra for the three targeted line-poor MYSOs and the hot-core source NGC 7538 IRS1. The organic-poor MYSOs have, as expected, a lower