• No results found

Birds exploit herbivore-induced plant volatiles to locate herbivorous prey

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Birds exploit herbivore-induced plant volatiles to locate herbivorous prey"

Copied!
35
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

1

Birds exploit herbivore-induced plant volatiles to locate herbivorous

1

prey

2

3

Luisa Amo1*, Jeroen J. Jansen2, Nicole M. van Dam3, Marcel Dicke4 & Marcel E. 4

Visser1 5

6

1 Department of Animal Ecology, Netherlands Institute of Ecology (NIOO-KNAW) , 7

P.O. Box 50, 6700 AB Wageningen, The Netherlands 8

2 Department of Analytical Chemistry, Institute for Molecules and Materials, Radboud 9

University Nijmegen, 6525 AJ Nijmegen, The Netherlands 10

3 Department of Ecogenomics, Institute of Water and Wetland Research (IWWR), 11

Radboud University Nijmegen, P.O. Box 9010, 6500 GL Nijmegen, The Netherlands 12

4 Laboratory of Entomology, Wageningen University, P.O. Box 8031, 6700 EH 13

Wageningen, The Netherlands 14

15 16

* Correspondence and present address: Luisa Amo. Departamento de Ecología 17

Funcional y Evolutiva, Estación Experimental de Zonas Áridas, CSIC. Carretera de 18

Sacramento, s/n E-04120, La Cañada de San Urbano, Almería, Spain. Telephone: 0034 19

950281045; Fax: 0034 950277100. E-mail: luisa.amo@eeza.csic.es 20

21 22

Running title: Birds smell herbivore-induced plant volatiles

23

Keywords: Multitrophic interactions, induced indirect plant defense, insect herbivores,

24

insectivorous birds, Parus major, foraging, apple trees, avian olfaction 25

(2)

2

Type of article: Letters

26

Number of words in the abstract: 149

27

Number of words in the main text: 4551

28 Number of references: 41 29 Number of figures: 5 30 Number of tables: 0 31

Number of text boxes: 0

32 33

Statement of authorship: L.A., M.E.V. & M.D. planned the experiments and wrote the

34

manuscript. L.A. carried out the experiments and took visual and chemical 35

measurements of trees. N.M.v.D. advised on volatile sampling and chemical compound 36

analysis. L.A. performed the statistical analysis of data, except the PLS-DA which was 37

carried out by J.J.J. 38

(3)

3 40

Arthropod herbivory induces plant volatiles that can be used by natural enemies of the 41

herbivores to find their prey. This has been studied mainly for arthropods that prey upon 42

or parasitize herbivorous arthropods but rarely for insectivorous birds, one of the main 43

groups of predators of herbivorous insects such as lepidopteran larvae. Here, we show 44

that great tits (Parus major) discriminate between caterpillar-infested and uninfested 45

trees. Birds were attracted to infested trees, even when they could not see the larvae or 46

their feeding damage. We furthermore show that infested and uninfested trees differ in 47

volatile emissions and visual characteristics. Finally, we show, for the first time, that 48

birds smell which tree is infested with their prey based on differences in volatile profiles 49

emitted by infested and uninfested trees. Volatiles emitted by plants in response to 50

herbivory by lepidopteran larvae thus not only attract predatory insects but also 51

vertebrate predators. 52

53 54

(4)

4 55

Among the most exciting interspecific relationships mediated by chemical cues 56

are multitrophic interactions involving plants, herbivorous arthropods and carnivorous 57

arthropods. When a plant is attacked by herbivorous arthropods, it induces a defense 58

response. The metabolites that the plant produces as a defense may directly affect the 59

performance of the herbivorous arthropod (induced direct defense) by, for example, 60

inhibiting feeding behavior of insects, decreasing digestibility or intoxicating the insect 61

(Schoonhoven et al. 2005). Furthermore, it has been proposed that the volatiles that 62

plants emit upon attack by herbivorous arthropods have an indirect defense function by 63

attracting carnivorous enemies of the herbivores (induced indirect defense, Dicke et al. 64

1990a; Turlings et al. 1990; Turlings & Tumlinson 1992; Vet & Dicke 1992). In doing 65

so, plants may reduce the damage by the herbivore, and thus, can enhance their fitness 66

(van Loon et al. 2000; Fritzsche Hoballah & Turlings 2001; Schuman et al. 2012). 67

The phenomenon of herbivore-induced emission of volatile organic compounds 68

by plants has mainly been studied considering insect enemies of the herbivores (see 69

Mumm & Dicke 2010; Dicke & Baldwin 2010 for reviews). However, many bird 70

species, such as the great tit, Parus major, are voracious predators of herbivorous 71

insects such as lepidopteran larvae, including the winter moth (Operopthera brumata, 72

Lepidoptera, Geometridae). Because the nestling period of the great tit coincides with 73

the peak occurrence of winter moth larvae, birds can greatly reduce the number of 74

lepidopteran larvae feeding on trees (Mols & Visser 2002). Predation of winter moths 75

by great tits has been found to decrease herbivore damage to trees (Mols & Visser 2002; 76

Van Bael et al. 2003; Mäntylä et al. 2011). This leads to increased growth and reduced 77

mortality of the trees (Marquis & Whelan 1994; Sipura 1999; Mäntylä et al. 2011). 78

Therefore, plants that are infested by herbivorous insects could benefit from the 79

(5)

5

attraction of insectivorous birds. Furthermore, insectivorous birds could also benefit 80

from the use of plant cues that enhance their chances to find their herbivorous prey. The 81

prey itself usually emits low amounts of cues thereby reducing detection by predators 82

(Rowland et al. 2008), whereas information emitted by the plant may be much easier to 83

detect due to the considerably larger biomass of plants compared to herbivores (Vet & 84

Dicke 1992). Previous evidence suggests that birds are attracted to trees infested by 85

lepidopteran larvae, without the need to see larvae or their damage on leaves (Mäntylä 86

et al. 2004, 2008a,b), but the mechanism underlying the attraction remains unknown. 87

Here, we present experiments aimed to elucidate whether birds are attracted to trees 88

infested by herbivorous prey and to explore the mechanism underlying such attraction 89

in the system: great tits - winter moths - apple trees. 90

In order to examine whether birds are attracted to trees infested by lepidopteran 91

larvae, we performed a two-choice experiment in an aviary (Fig. 1) containing two 92

types of apple trees, one control and one experimental tree. We investigated the first 93

visit and the proportion of visits by the birds to the tree that was experimentally infested 94

with winter moth larvae. We tested whether great tits preferred a) trees infested with 95

larvae, b) trees containing damaged leaves, from which larvae had been removed, or c) 96

trees infested by larvae, from which both larvae and the damaged part of each leaf had 97

been removed. If birds are able to use larva-induced tree volatiles we expected birds to 98

prefer the tree infested by lepidopteran larvae, even when larvae or the damaged leaves 99

had been removed before the choice experiment. 100

Next, we analyzed the mechanism responsible for the preference of great tits for 101

infested trees. We examined whether great tits were attracted to a) chemical cues, b) 102

visual cues, c) chemical & visual cues of apple trees infested with lepidopteran larvae, 103

from which damaged parts of leaves and the larvae themselves had been removed just 104

(6)

6

prior to the experiment. To further explore the potential cues used by the birds we 105

quantified the chemical and visual differences between infested and uninfested trees. 106

We expected that infested trees differed from the uninfested trees in the visual and 107

chemical cues that they emitted. Predatory arthropods are known to discriminate 108

between infested and uninfested trees based on the chemical cues that plants emit 109

(Schoonhoven et al. 2005). Despite the fact that birds are traditionally considered to 110

primarily use vision, recent evidence suggests that olfaction may be used more often 111

than previously thought, also in foraging contexts (e.g. Nevitt 2011). Therefore, we also 112

expect birds to, at least partly, rely on chemical cues to discriminate between infested 113

and uninfested trees. 114

115

MATERIALS AND METHODS

116

Insect species

117

In November 2006 and 2007 winter moth females (Operopthera brumata L.) were 118

captured in several deciduous forests to the west of Arnhem (05º48´E, 51º59´N), The 119

Netherlands. Females were kept individually in 50 mL falcon tubes (30 mm O.D., 115 120

mm length) to lay their eggs. Clutches were kept in petri dishes in outdoor conditions 121

until March, when eggs were transferred to climate cabinets (SANYO Incubator MIR-122

553) and maintained at 12 °C. Fresh young leaves of peach and apple trees were 123

provided to the containers to ensure that newly hatched larvae would have food. Larvae 124

were reared on these leaves until they reached the fifth larval instar (L5). 125

126

Tree species

127

From the beginning of April 2007 and 2008 we placed thirty-five 1.5 m tall apple trees, 128

Malus silvestris Miller (variety De Costa), planted in 40 L pots inside a greenhouse for 129

(7)

7

two weeks before the development of leaves. After leaf development, trees were moved 130

outdoors to habituate to experimental conditions. We separated control and 131

experimental trees several meters apart (minimum 10 meters) to avoid interactions 132

between them. 133

Three days before the experiment, we individually placed 30 winter moth (O. 134

brumata) larvae (L5) inside clip-cages (Ø = 250 mm) on each tree assigned the 135

“infestation” treatment. In this way, larvae could eat the leaf but could not move from 136

one leaf to another. We used 30 larvae because we wanted to mimic a natural situation, 137

where birds can find larvae in some but not in all tree leaves. Uninfested trees were 138

maintained without larvae. 139

140

Bird species

141

We used naïve captive adult great tits, Parus major, housed individually in 0.9 m × 0.4 142

m × 0.5 m cages. Birds were one year old and all of them were hand-reared since they 143

had been 10 days old; therefore, they did not have any previous experience in foraging 144

among trees. 145

Before the experiments, all birds were habituated to the aviary by releasing them 146

once during one hour inside the aviary without apple trees. In all experiments, we 147

removed the food from the cages that housed the experimental birds one hour before 148

each trial to ensure that the birds were motivated to search for larvae during the 149

experiment. After the trial, the bird was captured with a net and returned to its cage. 150

Birds did not show signals of stress during the trials and when they were returned to 151

their cages they immediately resumed their normal behavior. All experiments were 152

carried out under license of the Animal Experimental Committee of the KNAW (DEC 153

protocol no CTE 07.01). 154

(8)

8

We used thirty-eight adult great tits to test whether birds were attracted to trees 155

infested with lepidopteran larvae, and thirty-five other birds to examine the mechanism 156

underlying the discrimination between infested and uninfested trees. A repeated 157

measure design was used in both experiments. All birds were tested in the three 158

treatments in a randomized order. Only one trial was conducted per bird per day, and 159

there was at least one day without testing between trials. Before these experiments, 160

birds were trained five times to acclimatize them to the aviary and to allow them to find 161

larvae in the apple trees. During habituation trials, the mesh was partially removed (in 162

the experiment to assess the attraction to infested trees) or the door opened (in the 163

experiment to unravel the mechanism underlying the attraction to infested trees) to 164

allow birds to have access to both trees that were equally infested with larvae in these 165

trials. To maintain the birds’ interest to search for larvae during the trials, between each 166

trial of the experiment, we performed one habituation trial with each bird to allow it to 167

eat larvae from trees at both locations of the trees within the aviary simultaneously. 168

169

Experimental design

170

Experiments were performed in late April and early May in 2007 and 2008 in two 171

outdoor Y-shaped aviaries built with mesh screens (mesh size 1.3 cm) (Fig. 1). Each 172

branch of the aviary was 2.5 m x 2 m x 2 m (l x w x h). The central branch was closed 173

72 cm near the intersection with the other two branches. The aviary contained three 174

perches, one near each tree and the third in the middle of the aviary. 175

176

Experiment 1: attraction to infested trees

177

In this experiment, two apple trees were placed at the end of the branches of the aviary, 178

separated 4.40 meters from each other. One of the trees was uninfested and the other 179

(9)

9

tree infested. The infested tree had one of the following treatments: a) ‘Caterpillar’ 180

(infested tree with 30 lepidopteran larvae and damaged leaves), b) ‘Damaged leaves’ 181

(infested tree with damaged leaves from which the larvae had been removed), or c) 182

‘Previously infested’ (infested tree from which larvae and damaged parts of leaves had 183

been removed). The larvae and damaged parts of leaves were removed just before the 184

trials. We cut the damaged parts of leaves in the “previously infested” treatment. We cut 185

the part of the leaf that had been in contact with the larvae to remove not only the visual 186

damage but also any chemical compound left by larvae such as feces. We removed the 187

clip-cage containing the larvae by cutting the part of the leaf where the clip-cage was 188

located (about half a leaf). We also cut a similar part of the same number of leaves in 189

the uninfested trees and in the infested trees in the other treatments. Trees were covered 190

with protective mesh, to prevent birds from eating the larvae in the ‘caterpillar’ 191

treatment. We used 18 different pairs of trees. 192

193

Experiment 2: mechanism underlying the attraction to infested trees

194

Infested and uninfested trees (see above) were obtained as previously described. We 195

removed larvae and damaged leaves from infested trees and removed a similar number 196

of leaves in control, uninfested trees. Therefore, infested trees were similar to those of 197

the “previously infested” treatment in the former experiment. In this experiment, two 198

apple trees where placed at the end of each branch of the aviary and we thus had four 199

apple trees in the aviary (Fig. 1). Each pair of trees was located in a compartment with 200

two parts. One of the parts of the compartment contained a methacrylate door, and the 201

tree could be seen but not smelled. The other part of the compartment contained a cloth 202

(cotton) door, so the tree could be smelled but not seen by the birds. One of the pairs of 203

trees was control and the other one experimental. In the control pair of trees, trees were 204

(10)

10

always uninfested. The experimental pair of trees could have one of the following 205

treatments: a) ‘chemical’, b) ‘visual’, c) ‘chemical and visual’. In the ‘chemical’ 206

treatment, the tree that could be seen was uninfested and the tree that could be smelled 207

was infested. In the ‘visual’ treatment, the tree that could be seen was infested and the 208

tree that could be smelled was uninfested. In the ‘chemical and visual’ treatment, both 209

trees were infested, and therefore, birds could smell and see an infested tree. We used 210

12 different groups of 4 trees. 211

212

Experimental procedure

213

Trials were performed between 09:00 and 17:00 and under sunny and warm conditions 214

(mean + SE temperature = 20 + 1 °C) to avoid variation in the emission of volatiles due 215

to differences in ambient conditions such as temperature (Vallat et al. 2005). On each 216

test day, a new control and experimental tree or pair of trees was placed in each aviary. 217

We randomized the place of the trees (right or left) as well as the aviary (number one or 218

number two) among trials. Trees were tested with several birds (mean and median = 6 219

birds, from 2 to a maximum of 7 birds). We recorded the behavior of birds during 30 220

minutes using a video camera. An observer, blind to the treatments, analyzed the video 221

tapes and recorded the first tree inspected by the bird and the number of visits to each 222

tree during 30 minutes in the two experiments. We calculated the proportion of visits to 223

the control and experimental tree. 224

We analyzed the first choice as well as proportion of visits to the experimental 225

tree by using generalized linear mixed models fit by the Laplace approximation, with 226

these variables following a binomial distribution with logit function. The individual as 227

well as the tree pair were included in the model as random factors. Treatment, tree 228

location (left or right), aviary location (left or right) and order of trial were included in 229

(11)

11

the model as fixed factors, when relevant. Day, hour, hour2 and temperature were also 230

included in the initial models but were removed when they were not significant. 231

Treatment effect was calculated by comparing the models with and without the 232

treatment with ANOVA. Analyses were performed with the Statistical package R 2.15.1 233

(R Development Core Team 2012). Cases where birds did not visit any tree where 234

excluded from the analysis (two cases in the experiment to assess the attraction to 235

infested trees, and 23 cases in the experiment to disentangle the mechanism responsible 236

for the attraction to infested trees). 237

238

Chemical analysis through GC-MS

239

To further elucidate the mechanism underlying the attraction to infested trees, we 240

analyzed the volatile organic compounds emitted by 32 individual trees (16 infested and 241

16 uninfested) right after the behavioral tests, between 16:30-19:00 during 8 242

experimental days. We collected the volatiles of a subset of the total number of trees 243

that were used in the trials with birds (two infested and two uninfested trees each day). 244

We selected one branch of each tree with a similar number of leaves among trees and 245

introduced 20 cm of the branch into a 25x38 cm polyethylene oven bag (Toppits®, 246

Melitta, Lokeren, Belgium). To remove volatile organic compounds, the bags had been 247

heated for 4 hours at 120 ºC before use (Stewart-Jones & Poppy 2006). Bags were 248

fastened to the bark of the branch with tape and one of the two outermost bag corners 249

was cut to allow the placement of a tube containing a steel trap filled with 150 mg 250

Tenax TA and 150 mg Carbopack B. The trap was connected to a vacuum pump. 251

Collection flow rates were set to 200 ml/min. After 2 hours, the traps were removed and 252

capped till analysis. We also measured two background VOC profiles from empty bags 253

on two of the days. The values of compounds in these background samples were 254

(12)

12

subtracted from values in the tree samples. Traps were stored at 4 ºC for 10-11 weeks 255

until analysis. Volatiles were desorbed from the traps using an automated 256

thermodesorption unit (model Unity, Markes, Llantrisant, United Kingdom) at 200 °C 257

for 12 min (He flow 30 ml/min) and focused on a cold Tenax trap (–10 °C). After 1 258

minute of dry purging, trapped volatiles were introduced into the GC-MS (model Trace, 259

ThermoFinnigan, Austin, Texas) by heating the cold trap for 3 min to 270 °C. Split ratio 260

was set to 1:4 and the column used was a 30 m x 0.32 mm ID RTX-5 Silms, film 261

thickness 0.33 μm. Temperature program: from 40 °C to 95 °C at 3°C/min. then to 165 262

°C at 2 °C/min, and finally to 250 °C at 15 °C/ min. The volatiles were detected by the 263

MS operating at 70 eV in EI mode. Mass spectra were acquired in full scan mode (33-264

300 AMU. 0.4 scan/sec). Compounds were identified by their mass spectra using 265

deconvolution software (AMDIS) in combination with Nist 98 and Wiley 7th edition 266

spectral libraries and by comparing their linear retention indices. Additionally, mass 267

spectra and/or linear retention indices of chromatographic peaks were compared with 268

values reported in the literature. Additional confirmation for compound identification 269

was obtained by interpolating retention indices of homologous series, or by comparing 270

analytical data with those of reference substances. The integrated signals generated by 271

the AMDIS software from the MS-chromatograms were used for comparison between 272

the treatments. Peak areas in each sample were divided by the total volume in ml that 273

was sampled over the trap, to correct for small differences in flow rates over individual 274

traps. 275

We used an in-house written routine for Orthogonal PLS-DA for MATLAB 276

(Bylesjö et al. 2006), R2010a (Mathworks, Natick MA) to generate a model to describe 277

the general effect of treatment, by contrasting the chemical profiles emitted by trees in 278

the control group against those emitted by trees infested with lepidopteran larvae. The 279

(13)

13

data was log-transformed and the average emission of all trees per day was removed 280

from the data, to remove day-to-day variation caused by non-experimental factors. We 281

then determined the two latent variables for the model, by single cross-validation 282

(Westerhuis et al. 2008). We subsequently quantified the significance of the model 283

result by calculating the F-ratio of the obtained class predictions against those from a 284

permutation analysis, where factor ‘time’ was left intact but the ‘treatment’ factor was 285

permuted (Anderson & Ter Braak 2003). These showed that 1000 permuted models all 286

discriminated both treatments less well than that on the original data. We identified the 287

volatiles with largest OPLS-DA weights as significant for the treatment. This analysis 288

did not identify a significant change in the chemistry that underlies this difference 289

during the 14 days of the experiment. 290

291

Coloration measurements

292

We also collected five leaves from infested (N = 16) and uninfested trees (N = 15) used 293

in the behavioral trials and measured coloration. Color measurements were performed 294

by using a USB-2000 spectrophotometer with a DH-2000 deuterium– halogen light 295

source (both Avantes, Eerbeek, The Netherlands). During the measurement of each leaf, 296

we took three replicate readings and obtained the reflectance spectra of each 297

measurement. We calculated the total reflectance of leaves between 300 and 700 nm, 298

which include the spectral range visible to birds (320-700 nm, Cuthill 2006). We also 299

calculated the UV reflectance (between 300-400 nm) and human visible reflectance 300

(400-700 nm). Leaf color measurements were highly repeatable within leaves 301

(repeatability Total reflectance= 0.999; F154, 310 = 6.46; repeatability UV reflectance= 302

0.999; F154, 310 = 3.30; repeatability human-visible reflectance= 0.999; F154, 310 = 6.66) 303

and within trees (repeatability Total reflectance= 0.996; F30, 434 = 4.48; repeatability UV 304

(14)

14

reflectance= 0.998; F30, 434 = 2.35; repeatability human-visible reflectance= 0.996; F30, 305

434 = 4.61). Differences between infested and uninfested trees in the reflectance of 306

leaves were analyzed by using GLM with STATISTICA, controlling for the day as a 307 random factor. 308 309 RESULTS 310

Significantly more birds paid the first visit to the infested tree than to the control 311

uninfested tree (Fig. 2a). This preference for the infested tree was found in all 312

treatments (no differences in strength of preference among treatments (

χ

2 = 0.36, df = 2, 313

P = 0.83; Fig. 1a; significance levels for when the infested tree contained larvae and 314

damaged leaves: Z = 3.14, P = 0.002; only damaged leaves without larvae: Z = 3.08, P = 315

0.002; neither larvae or damaged leaves (“previously damaged”): Z = 3.40, P = 0.0007). 316

The birds also visited the infested tree more frequently than the uninfested tree during 317

the 30 min observation (Fig. 2b). Again, this was similar for all three treatments (

χ

2 = 318

2.67, df = 2, P = 0.26; “caterpillar”: Z = 3.77, P = 0.0001, “damaged”: Z = 4.01, P < 319

0.0001, and “previously damaged”: Z = 3.80, P = 0.0001), and thus the birds were 320

attracted even when they could not see the caterpillars or their feeding damage. 321

Furthermore, in tests addressing the cues used by the birds, their preference for 322

infested trees, measured as the proportion of visits, was only exhibited when the only 323

cues available were chemical cues (Fig. 3b; Z = 2.99, P = 0.003), but not when there 324

were only visual cues (Z = 0.77, P = 0.44; difference between chemical and visual cues 325

only:

χ

2 = 5.54, df = 1, P = 0.02). In contrast to the first experiment, in this experiment 326

the first choice did not differ between infested and uninfested trees (P > 0.29 in all 327

cases) or between treatments (

χ

2 = 0.12, df = 2, P = 0.94, Fig. 3a). 328

(15)

15

Leaves from infested trees differed visually from leaves from uninfested trees 329

(Fig. 4), with infested trees having a lower leaf reflectance than uninfested trees both in 330

the visual (F1,22= 9.32, P =0.006) and UV spectral range (F1,22= 4.80, P = 0.04). Trees 331

infested by lepidopteran larvae also differed from uninfested trees in their volatile 332

profiles (see table S1 in Supporting Information), as demonstrated by a validated Partial 333

Least Squares-Discriminant Analysis. They emitted more α-farnesene and dodecanal, 334

while they emitted less 1,2,4 trimethyl benzene, octen-3-ol, methoxy phenyl oxime, 1-335

nonene, and 3-octanol compared to control uninfested trees (Fig. 5). 336

337

DISCUSSION

338

Our results show that great tits exploit herbivore-induced plant volatiles to locate 339

herbivorous prey. The birds were attracted to trees infested by lepidopteran larvae, even 340

when we had removed the larvae and their feeding damage just before the experiment. 341

This allowed us to exclude the option that birds could see the larvae or their feeding 342

damage (Fig. 2). Thus, the preference for infested trees was not due to the visible 343

damage resulting from larval feeding on the leaves, nor by chemical cues associated 344

with the larvae such as silk or feces. A potential explanation for this is that these cues 345

may not accurately signal the current availability of prey in a tree. For example, the 346

presence of damaged leaves on a tree may cause an overestimation of the presence of 347

prey because the damaged leaves remain much longer on the tree than the larvae, which 348

could have been preyed upon or could have left the tree for pupation. 349

Our results show that birds can discriminate between infested and uninfested 350

trees based on the induced response of the tree. Our results are in accordance with 351

previous studies that recorded the attraction of passerine birds to infested trees (Mäntylä 352

et al. 2008a,b). In these previous studies, however, the cues responsible for the 353

(16)

16

attraction to infested trees were not separated and it was therefore not possible to 354

conclude whether visual cues, chemical cues or both were responsible for this attraction 355

(Mäntylä et al. 2008a,b). In contrast, we offered birds chemical or visual cues alone or 356

in combination. By doing so, we have shown that bird attraction to infested trees was 357

mainly mediated by chemical cues from the tree, i.e. bird preference for infested trees 358

was still exhibited when the only cues available were plant volatiles (Fig. 3), but not 359

when there were only visual cues. This demonstrates that birds were attracted by the 360

induced emission of volatiles by the tree rather than by the larvae themselves, the visual 361

damage caused by the larvae or the visual cues of undamaged leaves from the infested 362

tree. Similar findings have been reported in previous studies with predatory and 363

parasitoid arthropods (Dicke et al. 1990a; Turlings et al. 1990; Turlings & Tumlinson 364

1992; Vet & Dicke 1992; Mumm & Dicke 2010) but never for vertebrate predators. 365

Infested trees differed visually from uninfested trees (Fig. 4), with infested trees 366

having a lower leaf reflectance than uninfested trees both in the visual and the UV 367

spectral range. Therefore, the coloration of leaves could be a cue to ascertain the level 368

of herbivory of trees. However, visual cues may not be a reliable cue because the 369

reflectance of leaves may be related to other factors affecting trees rather than 370

herbivory, such as sunlight exposure (Mäntylä et al. 2008a). 371

Trees infested by lepidopteran larvae also differed from uninfested trees in their 372

volatile profiles (see table S1), emitting, among others, more α-farnesene compared to 373

control uninfested trees (Fig. 5). The sesquiterpenoid α-farnesene is present both in the 374

headspace of apple leaves (Takabayashi et al. 1991) and apple fruits (Boeve et al. 1996; 375

Landolt et al. 2000), and, at least for fruits, it is involved in the attraction of both 376

herbivorous and predatory insects (Boeve et al. 1996; Landolt et al. 2000). We show 377

that this compound is also present in the headspace of apple trees that are infested with 378

(17)

17

winter moth larvae and, thus that the birds can potentially make use of it when locating 379

infested trees. However, further research is needed to establish which compound or 380

mixture of compounds (Bruce & Pickett 2011) is responsible for bird attraction, as well 381

as to understand how differences in emission rates between infested and uninfested trees 382

modulate bird choice behavior. 383

The observation that birds use the volatiles from infested trees to find their prey 384

is in line with other studies on avian olfaction in foraging, and indicates that the 385

importance of olfaction in avian life history may be greater than was previously 386

thought. Phytoplankton releases chemicals to the seawater in response to zooplankton 387

grazing that is converted into dimethyl sulphide (DMS) that is emitted to the air 388

(Pohnert et al. 2007). Hence, DMS signals areas of high productivity in the ocean 389

(Nevitt 2011) and several species of Procellariiformes seabirds (Nevitt et al. 1995; 390

Nevitt 2011) and penguins (Amo et al. 2013) use DMS to locate these productive areas 391

(Nevitt 2011). Indeed, to use chemical cues during foraging seems to be an ancient trait 392

in birds (e.g. Kiwis Apteryx australis (Cunningham et al. 2009); Cathartes vultures 393

(Gomez et al. 1994)), and it persists in several modern lineages (Procellariiforms 394

(Nevitt et al. 1995); chinstrap penguins (Amo et al. 2013); zebra finches (Kelly & 395

Marples 2004); and domestic chicken (Marples & Roper 1996)). 396

The ability to detect the chemical cues emitted by infested trees may especially 397

be important for insectivorous birds such as great tits or blue tits that feed nestlings on 398

lepidopteran larvae, a resource that is variable in space and time (Perrins 1991) and is 399

abundant only for a very short period (Naef-Daenzer et al. 2000). Therefore, the 400

benefits for the birds of using induced volatiles from trees are obvious in terms of 401

increased fitness. From the infested tree’s point of view, the attraction of insectivorous 402

birds can greatly reduce the number of feeding larvae (Mols & Visser 2002), may be 403

(18)

18

beneficial in terms of decreased leaf damage and plant mortality (Mäntylä et al. 2011), 404

and therefore, may have a positive impact on fitness. Therefore, our results add to the 405

abundant literature showing that induced plant volatiles attract predators (reviewed by 406

Vet & Dicke 1992; Mumm & Dicke 2010), this time for vertebrate predators. This 407

novel evidence of the ability of insectivorous birds to use chemical cues of infested 408

plants to locate herbivorous prey is exciting because of the high predation rates of birds 409

compared to those of predatory arthropods. This further supports the incentive for plant 410

breeding to enhance the genetic trait underlying the induced volatile emission from 411

plants that are being attacked by insects (Dicke et al. 1990b) and in such a way 412

maximize the impact of insectivorous birds in the biological control of insect pests. 413

414

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

415

We thank Piet Drent for allowing us to use the birds for this study and for his helpful 416

comments. We thank Dr. Cornelis A. Hordijk for the GC–MS analyses of volatiles. We 417

are very grateful for their advice to Margriet van Asch and Leonard Holleman on the 418

rearing of winter moth caterpillars, and to Gregor Disveld on the care of apple trees. We 419

thank Marylou Aaldering, Floor Petit, and Janneke Venhorst for their help with the care 420

of great tits. LA was supported by the MEC postdoctoral program and by the Juan de la 421

Cierva program while writing, NMvD and MD by the Earth and Life Sciences Council 422

of the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research in the context of the European 423

Science Foundation EUROCORES Programme EuroVOL, and MEV by an NWO-VICI 424 grant. 425 426 427 428

(19)

19 429 REFERENCES 430 1. 431

Amo, L., Rodríguez-Gironés, M.A. & Barbosa, A. (2013). Olfactory detection of 432

dimethyl sulphide in a krill-eating Antarctic penguin. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser., 474, 433

277–285. 434

2. 435

Anderson, M.J. & Ter Braak, C.J.F. (2003). Permutation tests for multi-factorial 436

analysis of variance. J. Stat. Comput. Simul., 73, 85–113. 437

3. 438

Boeve, J.L., Lengwiler, U., Tollsten, L., Dorn, S. & Turlings, T.C.J. (1996). Volatiles 439

emitted by apple fruitlets infested by larvae of the European apple sawfly. 440

Phytochemistry, 42, 373–381. 441

4. 442

Bruce, T.J.A. & Pickett, J.A. 2011. Perception of plant volatile blends by herbivorous 443

insects – Finding the right mix. Phytochemistry, 72, 1605–1611. 444

5. 445

Bylesjö, M., Rantalainen, M., Cloarec, O., Nicholson, J.K., Holmes, E. & Trygg, J. 446

(2006). OPLS discriminant analysis: combining the strengths of PLS-DA and 447

SIMCA classification. J. Chemometr., 20, 341–351. 448

6. 449

Cunningham, S.J., Castro, I. & Potter, M.A. (2009). The relative importance of 450

olfaction and remote touch in prey detection by North Island brow kiwis. Anim. 451

Behav., 78, 899–905. 452

7. 453

(20)

20

Cuthill, I.C. (2006). Color perception. In: Bird Coloration. Volume I: mechanisms and 454

measurements. (eds Hill G.E. McGraw K.J. Harvard University Press, 455

Cambridge, pp. 3–40. 456

8. 457

Dicke, M. & Baldwin, I.T. (2010). The evolutionary context for herbivore-induced plant 458

volatiles: beyond the ‘cry for help’. Trends Plant Sci., 15, 167–175. 459

9. 460

Dicke, M., Van Beek, T.A., Posthumus,M.A., Ben Dom, N., Van Bokhoven, H. & De 461

Groot, A.E. (1990a). Isolation and identification of volatile kairomone that 462

affects acarine predator-prey interactions. Involvement of host plant in its 463

production. J. Chem. Ecol., 16, 381–396. 464

10. 465

Dicke, M., Sabelis, M.W., Takabayashi, J., Bruin, J. & Posthumus, M.A. (1990b). Plant 466

strategies of manipulating predator-prey interactions through allelochemicals: 467

prospects for application in pest control. J. Chem. Ecol., 16, 3091–118. 468

11. 469

Fritzsche Hoballah, M.E. & Turlings, T.C.J. (2001). Experimental evidence that plants 470

under caterpillar attack may benefit from attracting parasitoids. Evol. Ecol. Res., 471

3, 553–565. 472

12. 473

Gomez, L.G., Houston, D.C., Cotton, P. & Tye, A. (1994). The role of greater yellow-474

headed vultures Cathartes melambrotus as scavengers in neotropical forest. Ibis, 475

136, 193–196. 476

13. 477

(21)

21

Kelly, D.J. & Marples, N.M. (2004). The effects of novel odour and colour cues on food 478

acceptance by the zebra finch, Taeniopygia guttata. Anim. Behav., 68, 1049– 479

1054. 480

14. 481

Landolt, P.J., Brumley, J.A., Smithhisler, C.L., Biddick, L.L. & Hofstetter, R.W. 482

(2000). Apple fruit infested with codling moth are more attractive to neonate 483

codling moth larvae and possess increased amounts of (E,E)-α-farnesene. J. 484

Chem. Ecol., 26, 1685–1699. 485

15. 486

Mäntylä, E., Klemola, T. & Haukioja, E. (2004). Attraction of willow warblers to 487

sawfly-damaged mountain birches: novel function of inducible plant defences? 488

Ecol. Let., 7, 915–918. 489

16. 490

Mäntylä, E., Klemola, T., Sirkiä, P. & Laaksonen, T. (2008a). Low light reflectance 491

may explain the attraction of birds to defoliated trees. Behav. Ecol., 19, 325– 492

330. 493

17. 494

Mäntylä, E., Alessio, G.A., Blande, J.D., Heijari, J., Holopainen, J.K., Laaksonen, T., et 495

al. (2008b). From plants to birds: higher avian predation rates in trees 496

responding to insect herbivory. PLoS ONE, 3(7), e2832. 497

18. 498

Mäntylä, E., Klemola, T. & Laaksonen, T. (2011). Birds help plants: a meta–analysis of 499

top–down trophic cascades caused by avian predators. Oecologia, 165, 143–151. 500

19. 501

(22)

22

Marples, N.M. & Roper, T.J. (1996). Effects of novel colour and smell on the response 502

of naïve chicks towards food and water. Anim. Behav., 51, 1417–1424. 503

20. 504

Marquis, R.J. & Whelan, C.J. (1994). Insectivorous birds increase growth of white oak 505

through consumption of leaf-chewing insects. Ecology, 75, 2007–2014. 506

21. 507

Mols, C.M.M. & Visser, M.E. (2002). Great tits can reduce caterpillar damage in apple 508

orchards. J. Appl. Ecol., 39, 888–899. 509

22. 510

Mumm, R. & Dicke, M. (2010). Variation in natural plant products and the attraction of 511

bodyguards involved in indirect plant defense. Can. J. Zool., 88, 628–667. 512

23. 513

Naef-Daenzer, L., Naef-Daenzer, B. & Nager, R.G. (2000). Prey selection and foraging 514

performance of breeding Great Tits Parus major in relation to food availability. 515

J. Avian Biol., 31, 206–214. 516

24. 517

Nevitt, G.A. (2011). The neuroecology of dimethyl sulfide: a global-climate regulator 518

turned marine infochemical. Int. Comp. Biol., 51, 819–825. 519

25. 520

Nevitt, G.A., Veit, R.R. & Kareiva, P. (1995). Dimethyl sulphide as a foraging cue for 521

Antarctic procellariiform seabirds. Nature, 376, 680–682. 522

26. 523

Perrins, C.M. (1991). Tits and their caterpillar food supply. Ibis, S1, 133, 49–54. 524

27. 525

(23)

23

Pohnert, G., Steinke, M. & Tollrian, R. (2007). Chemical cues, defence metabolites and 526

the shaping of pelagic interspecific interactions. Trends Ecol. Evol., 22, 198– 527

204. 528

28. 529

R Development Core Team (2012). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical 530

Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna. 531

29. 532

Rowland, H.M., Cuthill, I.C., Harvey, I.F., Speed, M.P. & Ruxton, G.D. (2008). Can´t 533

tell the caterpillars from the trees: countershading enhances survival in a 534

woodland. Proc. R. Soc. B, 275, 2539–2545. 535

30. 536

Schoonhoven, L.M., van Loon, J.J.A. & Dicke, M. (2005). Insect-Plant Biology. 537

Oxford. Oxford Univ. Press. 538

31. 539

Schuman, M.C., Barthel, K. & Baldwin, I.T. (2012). Herbivory-induced volatiles 540

function as defenses increasing fitness of the native plant Nicotiana attenuata in 541

nature. eLife, 1, e00007. 542

32. 543

Sipura, M. (1999). Tritrophic interactions: willows, herbivorous insects and 544

insectivorous birds. Oecologia, 121, 537–545. 545

33. 546

Stewart-Jones, A. & Poppy, G.M. (2006). Comparison of glass vessels and plastic bags 547

for enclosing living plant parts for headspace analysis. J. Chem. Ecol., 32, 845– 548

864. 549

34. 550

(24)

24

Takabayashi, J., Dicke, M. & Posthumus, M.A. (1991). Variation in composition of 551

predator attracting allelochemical emitted by herbivore-infested plants: relative 552

influence of plant and herbivore. Chemoecology, 2, 1–6. 553

35. 554

Turlings, T.C.J. & Tumlinson, J.H. (1992). Systemic release of chemical signals by 555

herbivore-injured corn. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 89, 8399–402. 556

36. 557

Turlings, T.C.J., Tumlinson, J.H. & Lewis, W.J. (1990). Exploitation of herbivore-558

induced plant odors by host-seeking parasitic wasps. Science, 250, 1251–1253. 559

37. 560

Vallat, A., Gu, H. &. Dorn, S. (2005). How rainfall relative humidity and temperature 561

influence volatile emissions from apple trees in situ. Phytochemistry, 66, 1540– 562

1550. 563

38. 564

Van Bael, S.A., Brawn, J.D. & Robinson, S.K. (2003). Birds defend trees from 565

herbivores in a Neotropical forest Canopy. PNAS, 100, 8304–8307. 566

39. 567

van Loon, J.J.A., De Boer, J.G. & Dicke, M. (2000). Parasitoid-plant mutualism: 568

parasitoid attack of herbivore increases plant reproduction. Entomol. Exp. Appl., 569

97, 219–227. 570

40. 571

Vet, L.E.M. & Dicke, M. (1992). Ecology of infochemical use by natural enemies in a 572

tritrophic context. Annu. Rev. Entomol., 37, 141–172. 573

41. 574

(25)

25

Westerhuis, J. Hoefsloot, H.J., Smit, S., Vis, D., Smilde, A., Velzen, E.J., et al. (2008). 575

Assessment of PLSDA cross validation. Metabolomics, 4, 81–89. 576

577 578

(26)

26 579

Figures

580

Figure 1 Schematic representation of the aviaries used for the experiments. Numbers

581

indicate size in meters. This experiment was aimed to disentangle whether birds 582

detected the chemical or the visual cues of infested trees. Each pair of apple trees was 583

located inside a compartment with 2 compartments. The door of one compartment was 584

made of methacrylate to allow the bird to see the tree, but not to smell the tree. The door 585

of the other compartment was made with cotton material to allow the bird to smell the 586

tree, but not to see it. In experiments aimed to examined the attraction of birds to 587

infested trees, the same aviaries were used but without compartments. In these 588

experiments one tree was located in the same place that each compartment separated 589

from the rest of the aviary by a mesh. 590

591

Figure 2 Mean + SE of (a), Number of birds that paid the first visit and (b), Proportion

592

of visits to the experimental infested tree by great tits, Parus major (N = 38), when 593

released in an aviary with two apple trees: one control (uninfested) and one 594

experimental (infested). The experimental, caterpillar-infested tree had one of the 595

treatments: 1) tree with Operopthera brumata caterpillars feeding on the leaves 596

(‘caterpillar’), 2) tree with leaves damaged by caterpillars that were removed before 597

testing (‘damaged leaves’), 3) tree previously damaged by caterpillars but damaged 598

parts and caterpillars were removed before testing (‘previously infested”). 599

600

Figure 3 Mean + SE of (a), Number of birds that paid the first visit and (b), Proportion

601

of visits to the experimental infested tree by great tits, Parus major (N = 35), when 602

released in an aviary with two pairs of apple trees, one control (uninfested) and one 603

(27)

27

experimental (infested). The experimental tree pair had one of the following treatments: 604

1) Chemical cues, 2) Visual cues, and 3) Chemical and Visual cues (c.f. “Previously 605

infested” in Fig. 1) released by apple trees under Operopthera brumata caterpillar 606

herbivory. Caterpillars and damaged leaves were removed before the experiment. 607

608

Figure 4 Spectral analysis of leave coloration. Log-transformed mean + SE of (a), Total

609

Reflectance (300-700 nm), (b), human Visible Reflectance (400-700 nm), and (c), UV 610

Reflectance (300-400 nm) of control uninfested apple trees and apples trees infested 611

with Operopthera brumata caterpillars. 612

613

Figure 5 Chemical analysis. Log-transformed mean + SE relative emission rates (peak

614

area per ml volume sampled) of chemical compounds for which emission rates differed, 615

accordingly to PLS-DA, between control uninfested apple trees and apples trees infested 616

with Operopthera brumata caterpillars. 617

618 619

(28)

28 620 Fig. 1 621 622 623 624

(29)

29 625 Fig. 2 626 (a) 627 628 (b) 629 630 631

(30)

30 Fig. 3 632 (a) 633 634 (b) 635 636

(31)

31 637

Fig. 4

638

(32)

32

Fig. 5

640

641 642

(33)

33 643

Supporting Information

644

Table S1 Chemical measurements. Mean relative emission rates (peak area per ml

645

volume sampled) + SE of volatiles produced by uninfested trees (N = 16) and trees 646

infested with 30 Operopthera brumata larvae during 3 days (N = 16). Lri = linear 647

retention index. The identification of compounds marked with a * are confirmed by the 648

injection of reference compounds. 649

Compound Lri Uninfested trees Infested trees

2-pentenal 749 658 + 291 15195 + 780

acetic acid butyl ester 812 2567 + 691 7351 + 2649

1,3-octadiene 819 8748 + 7387 524 + 237 ethyl cyclohexane 827 97 + 48 143 + 50 furfural 828 143 + 52 263 + 133 2-hexen-1-ol* 863 3820 + 1558 25894 + 22155 1-hexanol* 873 11814 + 3977 30510 + 21791 1,2-dimethyl benzene 889 1848 + 651 955 + 497 1-nonene 890 23434 + 7987 11360 + 5974

propanoic acid butyl ester 909 2494 + 672 6636 + 2701

methoxy phenyl oxime 910 1354 + 525 637 + 372

tricyclene 916 39 + 22 63 + 20 α–pinene 927 7396+ 2353 3895 + 1022 gamma-valerolactone 949 477 + 154 101 + 42 benzaldehyde 952 5633 + 3431 9997 + 5135 1-heptanol 968 4675 + 2481 1849 + 758 benzene derivate 974 193 + 68 487 + 165 1-octen-3-ol 978 16438 + 13856 1691 + 736 1,5-octadien-3-ol 981 437 + 215 646 + 282 C3 benzene 988 2064 + 612 1019 + 407 2-pentyl furan 990 8037 + 3386 1731 + 833 6-methyl- 5-hepten-2-ol 993 1195 + 410 2157 + 916 3-octanol 996 749 + 358 223 + 143

butanoic acid butyl ester 997 425 + 142 1396 + 529

2-(2-ethoxyethoxy)-ethanol 1004 4947 + 2681 1722 + 591

2-hexen-1-ol-acetate 1018 4466 + 1538 18262 + 13487

2,6-dimethyl nonane 1021 829 + 244 402 + 207

benzene derivate 1022 858 + 478 265 + 84

(34)

34 2-ethyl-1-hexanol 1030 1428 + 803 4039 + 1393 benzyl alcohol 1032 230007 + 119913 121326 + 76838 2-methyl decane 1064 148 + 67 245 + 99 1-octanol 1071 21166 + 14497 4220 + 1818 C4-benzene 1072 122 + 42 59 + 27 methyl-benzoate 1090 594 + 224 255 + 97 4-nonenal 1095 2811 + 769 5112 + 2710 linalool 1099 96415 + 32543 167870 + 68320 benzene derivate 1111 61 + 22 116 + 38 2,6 dimethyl-1,3,5,7 octatetraene 1129 1017 + 381 1730 + 745 nopinone 1132 1336 + 483 724 + 319 benzene acetonitrile 1136 5082 + 2049 7636 + 2237 Z-3-hexenyl iso-butyrate 1148 1452 + 456 2906 + 1496 1-nonanol 1177 8124 + 4631 4647 + 1615 2-nonen-1-ol 1181 329 + 175 624 + 250 octanoic acid 1181 99 + 90 421 + 176 Z-3-hexenyl butyrate 1190 16990 + 5362 36891 + 14122 methyl salicylate* 1191 40180 + 20135 109821 + 52781 branched alkane 1266 250 + 104 528 + 174 1-decanol 1272 3049 + 1698 936 + 437 Z-hexenyl angelate 1280 995 + 363 1646 + 624 Z-3-hexenyl pentanoate 1285 281 + 97 369 + 145 Indole* 1287 8254 + 3492 15220 + 5934 naphthalene derivate 1299 133 + 41 70 + 21 branched alkane 1377 1240 + 474 663 + 214 bourbonene isomer 1380 404174 + 127253 205299 + 69867 β-cubebene 1387 2579 + 1151 1340 + 617 Z-jasmone 1392 6755 + 2499 18163 + 8969 dodecanal 1409 2304 + 929 4190 + 1161 copaene isomer 1432 821 + 392 446 + 203 allo-aromadenderene 1456 9235 + 4382 4235 + 1934 Z-cadina-1(6),4-diene 1461 11500 + 5360 6155 + 3130 D- germacrene 1477 320048 + 158792 95010 + 35948 1-pentadecene 1492 600 + 328 902 + 314 α–farnesene isomer 1 1495 184997 + 138483 51250 + 15825 α–farnesene isomer 2 1506 110191 + 40677 168574 + 69510 benzophenone 1620 57 + 37 170 + 80 isopropyl dodecanoate 1629 1006 + 453 4730 + 2195 1-tetradecanol 1675 210 + 81 418 + 189 cyclohexane undecyl 1763 12 + 9 58 + 29 benzyl-benzoate 1766 1708 + 781 3767 + 2980

(35)

35 hexadecanal 1819 723 + 255 1110 + 491 1-hexadecanol 1882 2605 + 2263 4122 + 2206 650 651 652

References

Related documents

For participants who enrolled with a CarePartner, those CarePartners received information sheets detailing the goals of the program, log books for tracking subjects’ symptoms, and

The aim of the study was to investigate the phenotypic correlations and the regression curve among the main phenotypic parameters of milk yield in a herd of 63

Suite à ces données, on s’est posé la question sur l’intérêt que peut apporter le dosage sérique et intracellulaire du protéasome chez les personnes âgées,

The Gridbus service broker [16] is a flexible open-source platform-independent resource brokering system, im- plemented in Java, which provides brokering services for

We find that high-tech oriented export policy and subsidies for S&amp;T activities have positive effects whereas local governments’ protection for local high-tech industries and

This study reports the first data on the prevalence and correlates of smoking blunts, cigarillos (Philly/Black &amp; Mild by brand name), bidis, kreteks, standard-sized cigars and

This, the literature shows, can often result in violence, particularly where rejection is a primary catalyst: in the setting of toxic masculinity, female rejection of male advances