• No results found

Treatment of Attachment Disorder of Infancy in a Neonatal Intensive Care Unit

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2020

Share "Treatment of Attachment Disorder of Infancy in a Neonatal Intensive Care Unit"

Copied!
6
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

EXPERIENCE

AND

REASON-Briefly

Recorded

“In Medicine one must pay attention not to plausible theorizing but to experience and reason together. ... I

agree that theorizing is to be approved, provided that it is based on facts, and systematically makes its deductions from what is observed. ...But conclusions drawn from unaided reason can hardly be serviceable; only those

drawn from observed fact.” Hippocrates: Precepts. (S/tort COfllflhlL?liCatio?lS of factual matt’rial are publisiwd liert’. Comments Out1 criticisms appear as latters to the Editor.)

Treatment

of

Attachment

Disorder

of

Infancy

in

a Neonatal

Intensive

Care

Unit

ABBREVIATION. NICU, neonatal intensive care unit.

As a psychiatric

consultant

in an inner-city

level

III

neonatal

intensive

care

unit

(NICU),

I have

seen

5ev-eral

cases

involving

premature

infants

who

did

not

have

a

caring,

consistent

adult

in

their

lives.

This

neglect

appeared

to contribute

to the

development

of

behavior

problems

(eg,

irritability)

or

feeding

prob-lems

(eg,

failure

to thrive),

and

sometimes

an

infant

failed

to progress

medically

or

experienced

an

exac-erbation

of medical

problems.

These

infants

were

as-signed

the

diagnosis

of reactive

attachment

disorder

of infancy.

This

is a recognized

psychiatric

entity

that

is defined,

as follows,

in the

Diagnostic

and

Statistical

Manual

of Mental

Disorders

(3rd

ed,

revised):

Definition: The essential feature of this disorder is markedly disturbed social relatedness in most contexts that begins be-fore the age of 5. The disturbance in social relatedness is presumed to be due to grossly pathogenic care that preceded the onset of the disturbance. Infants with this disorder present with poorly developed social responsiveness. Visual tracking of eyes and faces and responding to the caregiver’s voice may not be established by 2 months of age; attention, interest, and gaze reciprocity may be absent. The pathogenic care may include persistent disregard of the child’s basic emotional needs for comfort, stimulation, and affection. The clinical pic-ture can be substantially improved by adequate care. Such a therapeutic response is ultimate confirmation of the

diagno-Sis.

Associated features. Feeding disturbances may be present. There may be sleep disturbances, and hypersensitivity to touch and sound.

Age at onset. By definition, the age at onset is before age 5. The diagnosis can be made as early as in the first month of life. Course, inipairment, and complications. If care remains grossly inadequate, severe malnutrition, intercurrent infection, and death can occur.

Predisposing factors. All factors that interfere with early emo-tional attachment of the child to a primary caregiver can predispose to this disorder. Other factors that predispose to the disorder are lack of affectionate body-to-body contact dur-ing the first weeks of life, such as a prolonged period in an

incubator or other early separations from a caring adult.’

Prematurity

is not

specifically

mentioned

as a

pre-disposing

condition

for

the

development

of a reactive

attachment

disorder

of infancy,

but

predisposing

fac-Received for publication May 18. 1992; accepted Jul 15, 1992. Reprint requests to (M.S.G.) 30 N Michigan Ave. Chicago. IL 60602.

PEDIATRICS (ISSN 0031 4005). Copyright © 1993 by the American

Acad-emy of Pediatrics.

tors

such

as

a prolonged

period

in

an

incubator

or

other

early

separations

from

a

caring

adult

corn-monly

characterize

the

initial

months

of life

for

many

premature

infants.

Neonatal

medicine

is highly

sophisticated,

and

in-fants

who

never

would

have

been

considered

viable,

now survive.

These

are

the

infants

who

spend

weeks

and

months

in

our

NICUs

on

life

support

systems

with

close

monitoring

of each

breath

and

heartbeat.

Parents

are

understandably

fearful

of

these

infants

and

feel

sometimes

at

a loss

for

how

to

respond

to

them.

Because

their

infants

are

sick

and

often

sus-tamed

by

highly

technological

means,

these

parents

feel

that

the

usual

ways

of

showing

care

to

their

infants

is not

available

to them.

They

feel

not

needed

as

parents

as

well

as

frightened

by

the

NICU

atmo-sphere.

What

results

is

that

they

often

withdraw

from

their

infants.

Physicians

are

preoccupied

with

life-sustaining

measures

so

that

developmental

needs

are

often

given

low

priority

with

the

very

low

birth

weight

infant,

and

nurses

can

be

too

busy

with

routine

procedures

to

pay

constant

attention

to

the

attachment

needs

of the

infants

under

their

care.

As

a result,

many

of

these

infants

will

lie

for

several

months

in

their

isolettes,

receiving

state-of-the-art

medical

care

but

deprived

of the

emotional

care

that

is also

necessary

for

life.

The

following

cases

describe

four

premature

in-fants

who

had

no

interested,

consistent

caretaker

in

their

lives.

These

infants

developed

a reactive

attach-ment

disorder

of infancy

characterized

by

behavioral

problems

and/or

failing

physical

states.

They

are

ex-amples

of

the

need

for

interventions

in

the

NICU

which

address

the

developmental

needs

of

the

sick,

premature

infant

as well

as the

anxieties

experienced

by

the

parents

of

these

infants.

These

four

infants

illustrate

that

medical

care

is

often

not

successful

unless

accompanied

by

developmental

care.

Further

research

and

case

reports

are

needed

to understand

better

the

importance

of

the

integration

of

develop-mental

and

medical

practices.

Case

1

A baby girl who will be called Annie was born to a 16-year-old single mother after a 32-week gestation at a birth weight of 1475 g. Annie had a congenital jejunal atresia that would require two surgeries-a colostomy followed by a reanastomosis of the je-junum. I evaluated Annie and her mother when the infant was 2 weeks old. Her mother was 16 years old, and the neonatologist

wondered how she would cope with such a baby. Annie, at this time, was a responsive infant who made excellent eye contact with

whomever handled her. Annie’s mother seemed to like her; she

expressed fears about Annie’s small size, but she denied the

pres-ence of the colostomy and the need for a second surgical

(2)

140

EXPERIENCE

AND

REASON

as long as she could remember. She had never had a good rela-tionship with her own mother, and she hoped that she could give her baby everything she had missed in life. She had taped inside the infant’s isolette a picture of herself as an Il-year-old girl on Santa Claus’ lap; it was as if she had gotten into the isolette with Annie.

I saw Annie’s mother on three occasions. She then stopped visiting her infant. Annie, at 2 months of age, had her reanasto-mosis procedure, but she was having feeding problems. She was vomiting her milk. A postsurgical bowel obstruction was ruled out. In addition, behavioral problems had developed. Nurses com-plained to me that she was irritable and inconsolable. Now when I held her she maintained an alert state, but she would not make eye contact with me but would look away from my face. Her feeding intolerance, inconsolability, and gaze aversion character-ized the development of a reactive attachment disorder of infancy in an infant whose mother had withdrawn from her and who had no other consistent caretaker to hold her and talk to her. This particular NICU did not have primary care nursing. Intervention at this time was directed at both Annie and her mother. Her mother and I met, and we discussed her fears in caring for Annie, including her fear that Annie could die or would never be normal. When these fears were verbalized and understood, the mother began visiting and holding her infant. She visited several times a week, and those days she did not visit, she asked me to hold Annie. After 2 weeks, Annie had become a pleasant, related infant. She was less irritable; she liked being held and she maintained a gaze response with her caretakers. She also began to tolerate for-mula feedings.

Annie was seen in follow-up when she was 5 months of age. She was eating well, and she smiled, laughed, and babbled in a social interaction. She appeared to be a healthy baby. Her mother was pleased with Annie, but she reported she felt more like a baby-sitter than a mother. The mother’s difficulty in feeling like a mother would require further therapeutic intervention.

Case

2

Timmy was born at 31 weeks’ gestation at a birth weight of 1134 g. He was exposed to cocaine in utero, and he experienced all the complications of prematurity including respiratory distress syn-drome and severe bronchopulmonary dysplasia. He required me-chanical ventilation for a lengthy period of time. I was asked by the attending neonatologist to see Timmy when he was 4 months of age. He required high ventilatory settings and any attempt to wean him from his ventilator resulted in respiratory failure.

I observed Timmy in his isolette. He appeared to be a very unhappy infant. He thrashed about, arching his back, and he kept his eyes tightly shut, not focusing on anyone or anything. Nurses reported to me that this was his usual behavior, and nursing notes emphasized his irritability and frequent cyanotic episodes. Inquir-ies about family revealed that no one visited limmy and that he had never been held. The only touching he had experienced in his 4-month life was during routine tasks, such as diapering, or dur-ing medical procedures, such as respiratory therapy. Timmy had not experienced the nurturing and attention parents normally give their newborns, and he developed a reactive attachment disorder of infancy. All attempts to involve his mother in his care were

unsuccessful, and the family requested that the State assume re-sponsibility for him. Because there was no one else available, such as a trained volunteer, who was familiar with the behavior of this type of infant, I began to hold Timmy on a daily basis at the same time each day.

The first week ‘flmmy was restless in my arms, but when he was offered a pacifier, which he had not previously experienced, he gradually would become calmer and then mold to my body. After several days he opened his eyes and made fleeting eye contact with me. He was less irritable in his isolette, and his neonatologist began to decrease his ventilator settings. Three weeks after the intervention of daily holding began, Timmy pulled out his endotracheal tube. For the first time, he could breathe on his own with supplemental oxygen via a tot tent or nasal cannula.

By 6 months of age Timmy had become a responsive infant who would try to find me with his eyes when I approached his crib, talking to him, and he would lift his arms up as a signal of wanting to be picked up. His respiratory status remained fragile, especially at those times when he was not able to elicit a response from a caretaker. On two occasions he required mechanical ventilation for a 3-day period, and both these times followed a 5-day absence on

my part. A medical workup during each respiratory decompensa-tion was negative for findings. His physical well-being was de-pendent on my daily intervention of holding him and relating to him.

At 8 months of age, Timmy was transferred to a new unit within the hospital where he could have primary care nursing. He now related with lengthy periods of gazing into my eyes and lifting his hands to my face, and then he would look away when he had experienced enough stimulation. He was a playful infant, and we developed a repertoire of games, such as patty cake or play with a favorite toy. When I was away, another familiar caretaker would hold and interact with Timmy daily as I had been doing. Timmy was hospitalized until he was 21 months of age; five of these months were spent in a rehabilitation institute. He experi-enced fewer episodes of respiratory distress although he still re-quired oxygen via a nasal cannula. He was discharged to a foster mother who liked this babbling, smiling infant, and they devel-oped a relationship. limmy continues in his foster home to progress in all spheres of development-motoric, cognitive, and social-and at the time of this writing he is being weaned from his oxygen.

Case

3

Jessica was born at 28 weeks’ gestation with a birth weight of

794 g. This was the first baby for her 18-year-old mother and

slightly older father. Jessica initially required mechanical ventila-tion, and at 2 months of age, she began to be weaned from her ventilator. She was briefly in an Oxyhood, but then complications of sepsis and bilateral pneumonia necessitated a return to mechan-ical ventilation. A protracted medical course followed, during which time Jessica was a very sick infant. Jessica’s failing medical condition came to my attention when she was 3 months old. At this time I observed her in her isolette; she was an irritable infant who attempted to cry as she thrashed about. When I lightly placed

my hands on her head and trunk, she calmed and briefly alerted. When I removed my hands, she again became fussy. Nurses told me that the parents visited Jessica each weekend but they did not touch her or talk to her. I thought that Jessica’s failing medical condition could be symptomatic of a reactive attachment disorder of infancy. When I met with Jessica’s mother to discuss her feelings about her infant, she conveyed that she was a worried, concerned parent who would do anything she could for her daughter. How-ever, she explained how helpless she felt as a mother. She thought Jessica, right now, needed the doctors and the nurses and, as a mother, there was little she could do for her baby. I explained to the mother that she really could do a lot, that her voice, touch, and smell were very important for Jessica to experience. Her mother was able to see how peaceful Jessica became when she stroked her and talked to her and how her pulse oximeter reading quickly changed from 85 to 100. The mother began to visit Jessica several times in the ensuing week. Within days, Jessica markedly im-proved. In a week’s time she showed increased alerting behavior with eye contact while vigorously sucking on a pacifier. In less than 2 weeks’ time Jessica was extubated and breathing room air. Jessica’s mother now was holding her and Jessica would mold to her body while gazing into her eyes. Both obviously enjoyed this experience. She began to do well with her bottle feedings and after a 4 #{189}-month hospitalization she was discharged home at a weight of 2260 g. I saw both parents at the time of discharge. They were pleased withJessica and excited to be able to finally take her home.

Case

4

Billy was born after a 30-week gestation at a birth weight of 1134 g. He had a difficult first 6 months of life, with complications of bilateral pneumothoraces, pneumonia, and a grade I

intraven-tricular hemorrhage. He required a lengthy period of mechanical

ventilation. At 9 months of age he had a tracheostomy because of subglottic stenosis.

From birth, Billy’s mother, father, and maternal grandmother rarely visited him. His mother was 21 years old, and she had two other children, aged 2 and 3. Significantly, she had previously given birth to another premature infant who was cared for in a different NICU and who died at 7 months of age. His mother and grandmother agreed that they feared Billy, too, would die. Because they could lose him, they did not want to become close to him. Understanding these fears did not help to overcome them. Billy’s family continued to visit him infrequently, and his mother even-tually told me that she had become pregnant again.

at Viet Nam:AAP Sponsored on September 1, 2020

www.aappublications.org/news

(3)

When Billy was 8 months of age, his neonatologist asked me to evaluate his development. At this time, he was receiving supple-mental oxygen via a nasal cannula; he was feeding by bottle and spoon, and he weighed 3180 g. He had many caretakers in his life, and he had not developed a noticeable attachment to any partic-ular person. He was liked by the NICU staff, but lived in a busy, noisy unit that did not have primary care nursing. I observed Billy. He looked unhappy. He had a fixed stare, and he infrequently made fleeting eye contact with a caretaker. He did not smile or babble, and he could not roll over or reach for objects. He did not appear to enjoy his feedings, and he often refused his formula and baby food. Billy was a withdrawn infant with symptoms of failure to thrive, developmental delay, and halting medical progress. As a result of institutional life and the lack of a consistent, caring adult in his life, Billy had developed a reactive attachment disorder of

infancy.

I began to hold Billy daily. When held, he arched his body, became irritable, and usually looked away from my face. When he finally calmed in my arms, he would fall asleep, using the holding experience for soothing purposes. On occasion, he would interact by briefly gazing into my eyes, touching my face, or showing a small smile, but most of the time he appeared sad and lifeless. Attempts were now made to provide him with primary care nurses on the day shift, but Billy did not develop an intense attachment to either myself or any of his nurses. He continued to be a poor feeder, and he always weighed less than 4536 g. He looked like a marasmic infant. He frequently experienced

hypo-xemic episodes requiring increasingly high concentrations of

oxygen.

When Billy was 14 months old, he began a downhill medical course. He was more withdrawn, and he required increasingly high concentrations of oxygen and eventually mechanical venti-lation with 100% oxygen. Respiratory syncytial viral pneumonitis was diagnosed. At almost 17 months of age, he died. The major finding on autopsy was severe end-stage bronchopulmonary dys-plasia with fibrosis.

DISCUSSION

These

four

infants

became

symptomatic

when

they

did

not

experience

closeness

with

a consistent

care-taker,

and

a reactive

attachment

disorder

of infancy

was

diagnosed.

The

first

three

infants

showed

marked

improvement

in

their

physical

conditions

and

behavioral

symptoms

when

they

had

consistent,

predictable

attention

from

a caring

adult.

In the

sec-ond

case

this

was

a child

psychiatrist

and

in the

other

two

cases

the

infants’

mothers

fulfilled

this

role.

The

fourth

infant,

Billy,

who

received

intervention

at

8

months

of age,

never

really

responded,

and

he

even-tually

died.

All

four

infants,

as well

as

many

others,

raise

the

important

question:

What

are

the

develop-mental

needs

of the

premature

infant?

The

developmental

needs

of the

premature

infant

have

not

been

as well

understood

as their

physiolog-ical

needs

and

their

medical

problems.

Proper

stim-ulation

of

the

premature

infant

has

been

a

contro-versial

topic

for

decades.

The

adverse

effects

of

overstimulation

have

been

pointed

out

by

investiga-tors

such

as

Long

and

Lucey

and

Gorski.

Long

and

Lucey2

have

shown

that

sudden

environmental

noise

can

lead

to

crying

and

then

to

hypoxemia

and

ele-vations

in

intracranial

pressure,

a possible

precipi-tant

for

an

intraventricular

hemorrhage.

Gorski

et al3

have

shown

that

stress

created

by

mistimed

handling

can

cause

a bradycardiac

episode.

Als

et

al4

have

shown

that

those

infants

who

do

best

as evidenced

in

reduced

ventilator

and

oxygen

dependency,

earlier

success

in breast-

or bottle-feeding,

and

better

devel-opmental

performance

at 9 months

of age

have

been

shielded

as

much

as

possible

from

loud

noise

and

bright

light.

They

have

been

carefully

positioned

and

nested

to

enhance

motoric

and

state

organization

as

well

as self-regulatory

activity.

Some

of Billy’s

symp-tomatology

such

as his

frequent

hypoxemic

episodes

could

be

attributed

to the

brightly

lit,

noisy

environ-ment

in which

he

lived.

What

makes

for

proper

stimulation

of the

prema-ture

infant

has

not

been

as

well

defined.

A

round-table

of

infant

experts

has

underscored

the

need

for

further

interdisciplinary

research

to study

the

short-term

and

long-term

effects

of

infant

stimulation.5

Along

this

line,

the

attachment

needs

of

the

prema-ture

infant

requiring

interaction

from

a consistent,

caring

adult

have

not

been

understood

in

the

same

way

that

these

attachment

needs

have

been

studied

and

understood

in the

full-term

newborn.

Spitz6

was

one

of the

first

investigators

to document

the

effects

of

maternal

deprivation.

He

followed

infants

cared

for

in

an

institution

where

they

were

deprived

of

their

mother

and

a mother-substitute.

These

infants

showed

markedly

compromised

growth

and

devel-opment

as

well

as

frequent

infections

and

a

high

mortality

rate.6

He

observed

other

infants

who

were

separated

from

their

mothers

for

a 3-month

period.

These

infants

developed

symptoms

of

sadness,

weepiness,

withdrawal,

developmental

delay,

and

refusal

to

eat

and

loss

of

weight.

When

the

infants

were

reunited

with

their

mothers,

they

recovered

in

a sudden,

dramatic

way.

Spitz

and

cautioned,

though,

that

with

a long-lasting

separation

this

de-pressive

response

can

become

irreversible.

The

infant

in

case

4,

Billy,

could

have

experienced

too

much

deprivation

for

too

long,

so that

he could

not

respond

to the

maternal

care

that

finally

was

available

to him.

John

Bowlby,

a pioneer

in

the

field

of attachment

theory,

underscored

the

need

of

the

newborn

and

older

infant

to have

contact

with

a caretaker

for

the

purpose

of

closeness,

ie,

attachment,

and

indepen-dent

of the

fulfillment

of physical

needs

such

as

hun-ger.

The

full-term

newborn

signals

in

obvious

ways

his

need

for

human

contact

and

his

response

to that

contact,

ie,

crying

and

then

quieting

to

the

human

voice

or touch.8

Recent

infant

research

has

shown

the

importance

of the

mother

for

the

full-term

newborn.

Newborns

recognize

their

mother’s

voice9

and

smell,10

and

they

prefer

the

human

face

to an

inani-mate

object.1#{176}

Understanding

the

attachment

needs

of

the

pre-mature

infant

can

be

a more

difficult

task

than

un-derstanding

the

full-term

newborn’s

signals

for

closeness.

Often

the

premature

infant,

because

of

me-chanical

ventilation,

cannot

cry,

or

he

or

she

is not

able

to alert

and

gaze

(crying

and

gazing

being

sig-nals

that

mobilize

parental

or caretaker

proximity).

It

is not

uncommon,

then,

to

view

the

very

low

birth

weight

infant

as

too

fragile

to

benefit

from

interac-tion

with

a caring

parent,

which

is what

the

mother

in case

3 expressed

to me.

Investigators

are

attempting

to

understand

better

the

premature

infant’s

attachment

needs.

Als11

has

alerted

us

to

the

premature

infant’s

attentional/

interactive

system.

The

interactive

system

becomes

more

defined

with

increasing

gestational

age.

How-ever,

it is present

in

even

the

newborn

of

27

weeks

gestational

age.

Als

emphasizes

the

vulnerability

of

at Viet Nam:AAP Sponsored on September 1, 2020

www.aappublications.org/news

(4)

142

EXPERIENCE

AND REASON

the

premature

infant.

She

advises

careful

monitoring

of

the

infant’s

behavior

for

state,

motoric,

and

auto-nomic

changes

(drowsiness,

gaze

aversion,

color

changes,

breathing

difficulties,

hypertonicity)

which

signal

that

the

infant

has

had

enough

stimulation.11

Other

work,

too,

has

acknowledged

the

premature

infant’s

need

for

human

contact.

Field12

has

shown

that

infants

who

receive

massage

three

times

a day

gain

weight

faster,

show

more

organized

behaviors,

and

are

discharged

home

earlier

than

infants

who

do

not

receive

this

intervention.

The

Kangaroo

Method,

holding

the

infant

against

the

mother’s

bare

chest

and

breast-feeding,

is

now

used

in

some

NICUs.

Whitelaw

et

a113 reported

that

babies

who

have

ex-perienced

skin-to-skin

contact

with

their

mothers

cry

less

often.

VandenBerg

has

noticed

in

preliminary

results

of a study

that

premature

infants

with

bron-chopulmonary

dysplasia

who

are

held

daily

for

a

minimum

of 2 hours

by

the

same

volunteer

are

dis-charged

home

at an

earlier

date

than

infants

who

do

not

receive

this

intervention.

The

infants

who

were

held

achieved

better

state

control,

showed

more

alerting,

and

were

more

responsive

to

caretakers

than

the

control

infants

(VandenBerg

K,

personal

communication,

1991).

Our

premature

infants

need

consistent,

caring

adults

in

their

lives

to

touch

them,

hold

them,

and

talk

to

them.

Ideally,

these

caring

adults

should

be

the

infant’s

parents.

These

parents

require

help

to

feel

at

ease

in

our

NICUs

as

well

as

to

understand

how

they

can

play

a parental

role

with

their

infants.

Interventions

of this

type

will

enhance

our

state-of-the-art

medical

care

and

prevent

the

development

of

symptoms

characteristic

of

reactive

attachment

dis-order

of

infancy.

The

above

cases

and

my

observa-tions,

along

with

those

of

seasoned

neonatologists,

confirm

that

those

babies

who

have

caring

parents

at

their

bedsides

always

do

better

with

fewer

compli-cations

than

the

neglected,

abandoned

infants.

Vol-unteer

baby-holding

programs,

the

construction

of

NICUs

that

include

special

rooms

for

visiting

par-ents,

liberal

visiting

hours,

primary

care

nurses,

and,

in

one

NICU

that

I know

of,

primary

care

doctors,

attest

to increasing

understanding

of the

attachment

needs

of

the

premature

infant.

As

we

continue

to

understand

better

how

to

interact

with

our

prema-ture

infants,

it will

be

interesting

to

see

the

effect

of

this

developmental

care

on

their

progress

and

future

lives.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

I thank Paul C. Holinger, MD, Stephen L. Patt, MD, Steve Shel-don, DO, and the Rush-Presbyterian-St Luke’s Psychiatric Study Group for their helpful comments in the preparation of this paper.

MARLENE S. GOODFRIEND,

MD

Dept of Pediatrics

University of Health Sciences/The Chicago Medical School

Dept

of Pediatrics

Mount Sinai Hospital Medical Center Chicago, IL

REFERENCES

I. American Psychiatric Association, Committee on Nomenclature and Statistics. Diagiiostic tiiid Statistica! Manjia! of Menta! Disorders. 3rd ed,

revised. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1987:91-93 2. Long JG, Lucey iF. Noise and hypoxemia in the intensive care nursery.

Pediatrics. 1980;65:143-145

3. Gorski PA, Huntington L, Lewkowicz D. Handling preterm infants in hospitals: stimulating controversy about timing stimulation. In: Gun-zenhauser N, ed. Infant Stimu!atiou: For Whom, What Kind, When and Hozv Much? Johnson and Johnson Baby Products Company; 1987:43-51 4. Als H, Lawhon G, Brown E, et al. Individualized behavioral and

envi-ronmental care for the very low birth weight preterm infant at high risk for bronchopulmonary dysplasia: neonatal intensive care unit and de-velopmental outcome. Pediatrics. 1986;78:11 23-1132

5. Gunzenhauser N, ed. Infant Stimu!ation: For Whom, What Kind, When and How Much? Johnson and Johnson Baby Products Company;

1987:182-185

6. Spitz R. Hospitalism: a Follow-up report. Psyclioana! Study Chi!d.

1946;2:113-117

7. Spitz R, Wolf K. Anaclitic depression: an inquiry into the genesis of

psychiatric conditions in early childhood. 2. Psychoana! Study CJ,i!d.

1946;2:31 3-342

8. Bowlby J.Attacl:,nt’nt and Loss: Attaclnnt’nt, New York, NY: Basic Books; 1969;1 :265-298

9. Ockleford EM, Vince MA, Layton C, Reader MR. Responses of neonates to parents and others’ voices. Earhi Hum Dez’., 1988;18:27-36

10. Stern DN. TIir’ Iiit’rpersoiiaI Wor!d of the Infant. New York, NY: Basis Books; 1985:38-42

11. Als H. A synactive model of neonatal behavioral organization: frame-work for the assessment of neurobehavioral development in the prema-ture infant and for support of infants and parents in the neonatal inten-sive care environment. Pliysica! Occit;i Titer Pediatr. 1986;6(3/4):3-55 12. Field TM. Neonatal stress and coping in intensive care. Infant Mental

H,’a!ti,

J.

1990;2:57-65

13. Whitelaw A, et al. Skin to skin contact for very low birth weight infants and their mothers. Arc/i Dis Child. 1988;63:1377-1381

Dishwasher

Effluent

Burns

in

Infants

Burns

are

a significant

cause

of injury

in children,

and

hot

liquid

scald

is

the

most

common

form

of

pediatric

burn.1

The

depth

of scald

burns

depends

on

the

water

temperature

and

the

duration

of contact.2

To

our

knowledge,

scalds

in

infants

resulting

from

hot

dishwasher

effluent

being

forced

into

the

kitchen

sink

while

children

are

being

bathed

have

not

been

reported

previously.

A

specific

plumbing

arrange-ment

may

favor

the

occurrence

of

this

type

of

acci-dent.

Two

such

cases

were

managed

at

our

institu-tion

and

form

the

basis

for

this

report.

Case

1

CASE

REPORTS

A 6-month-old boy was being bathed in the kitchen sink by a care giver while the dishwasher was running. The child was sit-ting on a sponge ring over a sink drain that was occluded with a plunger-type drain cover. The child began crying suddenly and was lifted from the sink by the care giver, who noted that the dishwasher effluent was backing into the sink. The child suffered deep burns of his perineum and thighs, which required hospital

admission and 4 weeks to heal. The dishwasher drain entered the

top of a garbage disposal. Since that time, the parents have noted that when the garbage disposal is not completely clear, hot water from the dishwasher enters the sink during the drain cycle.

Received for publication Apr 1, 1992; accepted Jun 29, 1992.

Reprint requests to (R.L.S.) Shriners Burns Institute, 51 Blossom St. Boston,

MA 02114.

PEDIATRICS (ISSN 0031 4005). Copyright © 1993 by the American

Acad-emy of Pediatrics.

at Viet Nam:AAP Sponsored on September 1, 2020

www.aappublications.org/news

(5)

1993;91;139

Pediatrics

MARLENE S. GOODFRIEND

Treatment of Attachment Disorder of Infancy in a Neonatal Intensive Care Unit

Services

Updated Information &

http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/91/1/139

including high resolution figures, can be found at:

Permissions & Licensing

http://www.aappublications.org/site/misc/Permissions.xhtml

entirety can be found online at:

Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures, tables) or in its

Reprints

http://www.aappublications.org/site/misc/reprints.xhtml

Information about ordering reprints can be found online:

at Viet Nam:AAP Sponsored on September 1, 2020

www.aappublications.org/news

(6)

1993;91;139

Pediatrics

MARLENE S. GOODFRIEND

Treatment of Attachment Disorder of Infancy in a Neonatal Intensive Care Unit

http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/91/1/139

the World Wide Web at:

The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is located on

American Academy of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 1073-0397.

American Academy of Pediatrics, 345 Park Avenue, Itasca, Illinois, 60143. Copyright © 1993 by the

been published continuously since 1948. Pediatrics is owned, published, and trademarked by the

Pediatrics is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly publication, it has

at Viet Nam:AAP Sponsored on September 1, 2020

www.aappublications.org/news

http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/91/1/139 http://www.aappublications.org/site/misc/Permissions.xhtml http://www.aappublications.org/site/misc/reprints.xhtml http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/91/1/139

References

Related documents

If firms are free to choose the size of their installment, the incumbent has the largest incentive to invest first, for it can undertake a small investment in order to delay a

well as liver tissue TC and TG levels, decrease in serum total protein and albumin levels along with liver tissue marked oxidative and nitrosative stresses,

Thus, a training set of the 16 synthesized compounds were used in this study to construct a pharmacophore model using hydrogen bond acceptor, hydrogen bond

The changes in the shape of the prepared chitooligosaccharides groups were studied by using scanning electron microscope (SEM).The shape and size of the obtained

We invite unpublished novel, original, empirical and high quality research work pertaining to recent developments & practices in the areas of Computer Science &

So given the margins of error in these data, can any of the smaller Lancaster County communities demonstrate with confidence that the level of family poverty found there exceeds

Extended Enterprise, Lean production, Supply Chain Management, Automotive Value Chain, Logistic model, Forrester effect, Transparency, Visualisation...