• No results found

Early Contact and Maternal-Infant Bonding: One Decade Later

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2020

Share "Early Contact and Maternal-Infant Bonding: One Decade Later"

Copied!
8
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

Early

Contact

and

Maternal-Infant

Bonding:

One

Decade

Later

Michael E. Lamb, PhD

From the Department of Pediatrics, University of Utah Medical School, Salt Lake City

ABSTRACT. In the last decade, there has been

consid-erable speculation concerning the importance of early skin-to-skin contact between partunent mothers and neo-nates. This contact is viewed as crucial to the occurrence of maternal bonding, which is seen as a precursor of optimal maternal behavior and thus as a necessary ante-cedent of optimal child development. In the present re-view, these conclusions are shown to have been based on

equivocal findings obtained in methodologically

impov-erished studies. Although early contact may have modest but beneficial short-term effects in some circumstances, no positive long-term effects have been demonstrated.

Pediatrics 70:763-768, 1982; bonding, attachment, early

contact, maternal behavior.

In the last decade, considerable excitement has

been generated by the suggestion that events

oc-cutTing in the immediate postpartum period could

have a substantial influence on parental behavior

and thus on subsequent child development. Klaus

and co-workers’ and de Chateau57 have argued

that there exists in human mothers a sensitive

period immediately after delivery during which

cer-tam experiences are more likely to produce

affec-tionate attachments or bonds to infants than at any

other time. When this “bonding” process is

inter-rupted, warned Klaus and Kennell,’ various forms

of aberrant parental behavior, including child abuse

and neglect, are more apt to occur and suboptimal

child development is more likely. Such claims

pro-vided the scientific basis for changes in accepted

obstetric procedures, and we owe a great deal to Klaus and Kennell for facilitating the humanization

of birthing practices. It is important, however, to

evaluate the evidence upon which their argument

was based, because their conclusions have

far-reaching implications for understanding the

deter-Received for publication May 7, 1982; accepted June 16, 1982. Reprint requests to (MEL.) Department of Pediatrics, Univer-sity of Utah Medical School, Salt Lake City, UT 84132. PEDIATRICS (ISSN 0031 4005). Copyright © 1982 by the American Academy of Pediatrics.

minants of human behavior and development. That

is the goal of this paper. For brevity, I will not

discuss either (1) the determinants of maternal behavior in other species, although I have argued

elsewhere8 that the analogy drawn by Klaus and

Kennell is flawed in many respects, or (2) research

on early contact with preterm infants, inasmuch as

these studies involved contact after the

hypothe-sized sensitive period. Readers are referred to a

longer review8 for fuller details, especially those

concerning the manner in which mothers in the

various groups were treated. Unless

otherwise

noted, control group mothers were denied contact

with their nude infants.

SHORT-TERM CONSEQUENCES OF EARLY

AND EXTENDED CONTACT

In the first study, Klaus et al2 allowed a group of

14 poor, predominantly black, unmarried,

primip-aras one hour of contact with their nude newborns

during the first

three

hours after delivery. These

mothers were also given their babies for five

addi-tional hours on each of the next three days. One

month after delivery, the mothers who had been

allowed

extended

contact were reportedly more

re-luctant to leave their infants with baby-sitters, more

attentive during a pediatric examination, soothed

their

babies more, and engaged in more fondling

and “en face” behavior than did mothers in the

control group. These fmdings provide very little

support for claims regarding the beneficial effects

of early contact for several reasons. First, only a

small proportion of the measures (five out of at

least 75) revealed significant differences, so most

may have occurred by chance. Thus we can only

consider the findings reliable if they can be

repli-cated by other researchers. Second, early contact

was confounded with extended contact. Third, the

nursing and medical staff members (including the

physician who conducted the pediatric

(2)

contact group were special, and they are likely to

have been treated differently as a result.

In a later study conducted in Guatemala, the

same researchers compared three groups: mothers

receiving routine care (first contact with clothed

baby at 12 hours); mothers who had 45 minutes of

skin-to-skin contact 12 hours post partum; and

mothers who had 45 minutes of skin-to-skin contact immediately after birth.9”#{176} Thirty-six hours after

delivery, mothers who had early contact engaged in

more en face behavior (and thus seemed more

af-fectionate) than mothers in the control groups.

Although only one of the 19 behavioral measures revealed a significant difference, the specific

differ-ence reported replicates the finding reported by

Klaus et al2 and this makes the finding more

im-pressive.

In another series of studies, de Chateau and

Wiberg5”’3 allowed 22 middle-class Swedish

pri-miparas and their infants 15 minutes of extra

skin-to-skin contact shortly after delivery during which

a midwife helped the mothers establish nursing.

These women were then compared with 20

primip-aras who experienced routine treatment which

in-volved no skin-to-skin contact. Thirty-six hours

after delivery, the groups differed on three of 35

measures (mother sitting up, leaning on elbow, and

holding infant) all three of which were measures on

which Hales et al’#{176}found no group differences.

These differences do not indicate that the mothers

who experienced early contact were more closely

bonded, more competent, or more affectionate, and

they were found only among the mothers of boys.

Furthermore, the babies in the control group cried

a great deal more during the observation, leading

one to wonder whether group differences were

at-tributable to differences in the infants’ behavior. In

sum, the effects revealed in this study were of

modest magnitude, unrelated to maternal

attach-ment, and failed to replicate findings obtained in

other studies.

Three research teams have attempted to

distin-guish between the effects of early and extended

contact. First, Hopkins and Vietze’4”5 assigned 104

lower income primiparas and their newborns to one

of four treatment conditions: early contact;

ex-tended contact (rooming-in); both early contact and

rooming-in; and routine care. Some group

differ-ences were evident in observations two days after

delivery, but there were no differences in

affection-ate behavior. The number of significant group

dif-ferences also seems to have been less than would

be expected to occur by chance. Second, Grossman

et al’6 compared the behavior of primiparous

Ger-man mothers in four similar groups. Mothers in the

early contact groups showed more tender touching

and cuddling on days 2 or 3 and 4 or 5 than did

mothers in the extended contact and control groups.

However, these differences were only observed in

mothers who had planned the pregnancy, and there

were no group differences seven to nine days post

partum. At no point did extended contact appear to

have a significant effect. Third, Carlsson et al’7”8

and Hwang’9 observed Swedish primiparas from

diverse socioeconomic backgrounds breast-feeding

their infants on the second and fourth days after

delivery. Some of the mothers had been allowed to

keep their naked infants for one to two hours

im-mediately after delivery, whereas others

experi-enced the regular hospital routine. Whether or not

they were allowed extended contact, mothers who

experienced early contact showed more contact (ie,

behaviors involving physical contact) and fewer

noncontact behaviors during both observations. No

significant group differences emerged on those

mea-sures (eg, en face, touching) on which other

re-searchers have found significant differences. Thus

we have a specific failure to replicate, but some

support for the value of early contact. These three

studies thus suggest that early (although not

ex-tended) contact has a short-lived beneficial effect

on some mothers. Other studies, however, fail to

substantiate this.

Taylor et al2#{176}observed two groups of middle-class

mothers who were allowed different amounts of

exposure to their infants after delivery. There were

no differences between the groups at two and 30

days post partum on measures of: the amount of

time mother and infant spent together; reported

maternal attachment; the mothers’ perceptions of

their infants; maternal behavior during feedings at

2 and 30 days of age; maternal sensitivity to infant

needs; and reluctance to leave the infants with

baby-sitters. Mothers and sons (but not daughters)

who had early contact were rated higher on “quality

of interaction.” Unfortunately, this study provides

only a weak test of the hypothesis of Klaus and

Kermell because: (1) the mothers in both groups

had some early contact; (2) the extra contact in the

early contact group only began 30 minutes post

partum; and (3) skin-to-skin contact was not

pro-vided because the infants were clothed.

In another recent study, Svejda et al2’ observed

30 middle-class primiparas and their infants 36

hours after delivery. Half of the mothers and infants

experienced roughly the same contact as those in

the study group of de Chateau.5 When observed

interacting with and breast-feeding their infants 36

hours after delivery, however, mothers given extra

contact and routine hospital care did not differ on

any of 28 specific measures or four composite

mea-sures, which included most of the variables used in

previous studies of infant-mother bonding.2”#{176}”

(3)

Curry,22 albeit in a small study involving only 11

control mothers and nine study mothers allowed at

least 15 minutes of contact with their naked infants

within the first hour. These conditions were similar

to those in the study of de Chateau and Wiberg,”

but there were no differences between the two

groups of mothers at 36 hours on any of the

mea-sures used by de Chateau and Wiberg.

Finally, Anisfeld and Lipper3 compared mothers

allowed one hour of contact at delivery with

moth-ers accorded the normal hospital routine. During a

feeding two days later, mothers who experienced

early contact were more affectionate. However,

they also reported receiving more social support,

which suggests that the groups may not have been

comparable initially.

LONG-TERM CONSEQUENCES OF EARLY AND

EXTENDED CONTACT

On the assumption that short-term effects were

well established, many researchers have attempted

to discern long-term effects. In a one-year

follow-up of their subjects, Kennel! et al24 reported that

mothers who had been allowed early contact with

their infants appeared more concerned and

atten-tive during a pediatric examination than did

moth-ers in the control group. The mothers who

experi-enced early contact also reported being more

anx-jous and more reluctant to leave their infants with

baby-sitters. On an unknown but apparently exten-sive number of behavioral measures scored during

free play, picture-taking, interview, and Bayley test

sessions, however, mothers in the two groups did

not differ. In all, therefore, the vast majority of the

measures revealed no group differences. It is

im-pressive, however, that the attitudinal differences

evident at one month post partum stifi

distin-guished the groups at one year.

Five of the mothers in each group returned for

assessments of maternal speech patterns when the

children were aged 1 and 2 years.25 At one year post

partum, the control group mothers used more

state-ments. A year later, the mothers in the study group

asked more questions, and used more words per

proposition, fewer content words, more adjectives,

and fewer commands in addressing their children

than did the mothers in the control group. Given

the sample size, these must be considered as pilot

data, rather than serious experimental evidence.

The findings, in any event, only show that the

speech patterns differed, not that they were better

or worse.

At 5 years of age, 19 of the 28 children took three

tests of intellectual and linguistic functioning.26’27 In

the study group, a few measures of maternal speech

were significantly correlated with measures of the

child’s linguistic competence, whereas these

corre-lations were not significant in the control group.27

Because the ratio of significant differences to the

number of statistical tests was low (7: 140) there

was, at best, weak support for the claim that

ma-ternal characteristics were associated with the

child’s competence only when mother and infant

experienced early contact. However, the

associa-tions between maternal and child measures did not

involve facilitation of the children’s development,

as there were no group differences in performance

on any test.

In their follow-up, de Chateau and Wiberg’2

found that the early contact mothers kissed and

held their 3-month-old infants more in the en face

position and cleaned their infants less than did

mothers in the control group. In addition, infants in

the early contact group cried less and smiled!

laughed more than infants in the control group. Of

61 measures, only these five revealed significant

group differences. Once again the effects were

strongest for mothers and sons, and the

interpre-tation is again unclear: Could differences in infant

behavior during the observations have accounted

for the differences in maternal behavior? Did early

contact create happier dyads? Or were the infants

in the control group temperamentally more irritable

(remember that they cried more at both 36 hours

and 3 months of age) and thus produced differences

in maternal behavior? In any event, Curry22 was

unable to replicate these findings in an otherwise

comparable study.

One year post partum, the early contact mothers

studied by de Chateau and Wiberg28 held their

infants closely more often, engaged in more

non-functional touching, and talked more warmly than

did the control group mothers. On standardized

tests, the children who had received early contact

were not significantly more advanced

developmen-tally at 1 or 3 years of age.7’29’3#{176}Compared with

control mothers, early contact mothers also

re-ported more accelerated language development, less

eagerness to return to work, less paternal

involve-ment, less willingness to give infants rooms of their

own, and earlier bladder training. The meaning of

these differences is unclear. Further, it seems that

only a chance number of the many items assessed

revealed significant group differences. In their

fol-low-up, Carisson and colleagues’7’3’ found that the

effects of early contact evident two and four days

post partum disappeared by the time the infants

were 6 weeks old.

Au and Lowry32 studied 100 lower-class Jamaican

women, half of whom had 45 minutes of contact

immediately after birth, whereas the others first

experienced contact nine hours post partum.

Moth-ers were observed during clinic examinations when

(4)

weeks, there were no group differences in maternal

behavior, whereas at age 12 weeks, mothers in the

study group were more likely to stand and watch

the examination, more likely to look at their infants

while feeding them, and more likely to vocalize to

them. These differences may suggest that mothers

in the early contact group felt more comfortable

with the examining physician; she or he was not

blind to experimental condition and may well have

behaved differently toward mothers in the two

groups. Inasmuch as the differences were not

evi-dent at six weeks post partum, furthermore, it seems

unreasonable to attribute them to differences in

early contact. There were no group differences in

the mothers’ reported willingness to leave their

infants with baby-sitters or in the likelihood of

breast-feeding, but the early contact mothers were

less likely to use supplementary foods. Overall, it

seems that the four significant group differences

were derived from 150 comparisons, which is less

than would be expected by chance.

In one of the largest attempts to assess the effects

of both early and extended contact, Siegel et al

studied 321 women, most of whom were black and

unmarried. At both four and 12 months post

par-tum, observations and interviews yielded 30 specific

behavioral items and 92 ratings which were then

factor analyzed. At four months, early and extended

contact accounted for only 2.8% of the variance in

“maternal acceptance” and 3.3% of the variance in

“consoling.” At 12 months post partum, early and

extended contact accounted for only 3.2% of the

variance in “infant positive/negative behavior.”

Early and extended contact did not affect the

like-lihood of reported abuse or the utilization of health

care services. Contact thus appeared to have a

trivial effect on maternal behavior.

By contrast, O’Connor reported that, by 17

months, more routine care mothers provided

sub-optimal care than mothers who had roomed with

their infants. The low incidence of child abuse

makes the finding difficult to evaluate, however,

especially as it was not replicated by Siegel et al.

In several later assessments of the subjects of

Hopkins and Vietze’5 no differences in affectionate

behaviors were reported.36’37 Group differences,

when found, were not consistent from assessment

to assessment and an insignificant number of the

many (>100) measures revealed group differences.

Again, extended contact in the lying-in period did

not have long-term effects.

Anisfeld and Lipper23 observed and interviewed

their subjects again three months after delivery.

They found that the early contact mothers reported

being less eager to place their babies on schedules

and more likely to pick up their babies when they

cried. Taylor et al,20’37’ however, found that early

contact had no effect on maternal perceptions of

infant temperament at age 8 months and had no

effect on the security of attachment at age 12

months. Similarly, in their follow-up, Grossman et

al39 found that early contact had no effect on the

security of attachment. Rode et a14#{176}likewise found

that mother-infant separations had no effect on the

security of attachment among preterm infants.

By contrast, Kontos4’ reported that, when

ob-served in free play at home when the infants were

1 and 3 months old, middle-class mothers who had

early contact scored much higher on a composite

“maternal attachment” index at both ages than did

mothers in a routine care comparison group.

Unfor-tunately, all observations were conducted by the

author, who was aware of the hypotheses and of the

mothers’ group status. The potential for bias was

thus great.

Instead of the quality of mother-infant

interac-tion, several researchers have examined the effects

of early contact on breast-feeding. Sosa et al42

con-ducted three such studies involving primiparous

Guatemalan women. In all three, mothers in the

experimental group nursed their nude babies on the

delivery table for 45 minutes before a 12- to 24-hour separation. In the first study, mothers in the control

group continued breast-feeding significantly longer;

in the second study, the mothers in the study group

breast-fed nonsignificantly longer. Unfortunately,

the mothers in the control and study groups came

from different socioeconomic backgrounds. In the

third, smaller, study, the early contact mothers

breast-fed significantiy longer than mothers in the

control group. Only 25% of the mothers in the

control group v.’ere still breast-feeding at five

months post partum, however, whereas a larger

survey revealed that 50% usually continued

breast-feeding this long. Less than 60% of the study group

mothers continued breast-feeding five months,

which suggests that, relative to the common

cul-tural practices, early contact had, at best, a modest

effect on breast-feeding.

In Brazil, meanwhile, Sousa et al4t reported that

mothers who were allowed to suckle their infants

right after delivery and then “roomed-in” with their

infants were much more likely to continue

breast-feeding for at least two months (77%) than were

mothers accorded the normal hospital routine

(28%). These results are compromised, however,

inasmuch as a “friendly nurse” encouraged and

provided advice about breast-feeding to the

moth-ers in the early contact group but not to those in

the control group.

Several other studies have explored the effect of

early contact on breast-feeding. Johnson44 reported

that mothers who nursed during the first hour were

(5)

partum than were mothers who first breast-fed (and

handled) their infants at 16 hours of age. However,

Carisson et al,3’ Vietze et al,36 and Taylor et al#{176}

reported no effects on breast-feeding. De Chateau

and co-workers7’29’45 reported that early contact

pro-longed breast-feeding, but unfortunately there were

prenatal group differences in attitudes toward

par-enting, and the early contact mothers were assisted

in breast-feeding.

SUMMARY

The preponderance of the evidence thus suggests

that extended contact has no clear effects on

ma-ternal behavior. By contrast, some researchers seem

to have shown that early contact has positive

short-term effects. This conclusion appears premature,

however, as serious methodologic problems

com-promise the research. First, there is the problem of

nonreplication. Even though many studies report

positive consequences, they usually fail to find

ef-fects on similar measures, even when the same

subjects are followed over time. Otherwise

compa-rable studies often fail to demonstrate similar

ef-fects.

Second, some of the behavioral differences

ob-served appear unrelated to maternal attachment.

For example, de Chateau and Wiberg” in their early

(36 hour) assessments only found differences on

measures of maternal posture that bear no

relation-ship to maternal attachment.

Third, many of the researchers have employed

multiple measures and observed statistically

signif-icant differences on a small proportion of these:

generally, the number of significant group

differ-ences usually hovers around the number that would

be expected to occur by chance.

Fourth, many of the studies concerned with

ef-fects on breast-feeding are confounded by

encour-agement or coaching for mothers in the study group.

Other studies report no effects. Even if effects were

found, they would not demonstrate that early

con-tact affects maternal attachment, for although

breast-feeding may facilitate bonding, it is not an

index thereof.46

Fifth, it is not clear that any effects observed are

accounted for by early contact rather than

differ-ences in treatment by medical and nursing staff

who know that the study group subjects are special.

Finally, preexisting group differences, as in the

Guatemalan sample of Sosa et al42 or the Swedish

sample of de Chateau,7 seem as likely as the

treat-ment itself to account for the reported “treatment

effects.”

In sum, claims regarding the effects of early

con-tact on maternal-infant bonding are not well

sup-ported by the empirical evidence. Early contact has

no enduring effects on maternal attachment, but

may sometimes have modest short-term effects on

some mothers in some circumstances.

Unfortu-nately, we do not know what these characteristics

might be, as the evidence is inconsistent or lacking.

There is no support for the claim47 that early

con-tact is especially beneficial for young, poor, socially isolated mothers.

REFERENCES

1. Klaus MH, Kennell JH: Maternal.Infant Bonding. St Louis,

CV Mosby, 1976

2. Klaus MH, Jerauld R, Kreger NC, et a): Maternal

attach-ment: Importance of the first postpartum days. N EngI J

Med 286:460, 1972

3. Kennell JH, Voos DK, Klaus MH: Parent-infant bonding, in

Osofsky JD (ed): Handbook of Infant Development. New York, John Wiley & Sons, 1979

4. Lozoff B, Brittenham GM, Trause MA, et a!: The mother-newborn relationship: Limits of adaptability. J Pediatr 91:1,

1977

5. de Chateau P: Neonatal Care Routines: Influences on Ma-ternal and Infant Behaviour and on Breastfeeding, thesis.

Ume#{225},Sweden, 1976

6. de Chateau P: The importance of the neonatal period for the development of synchrony in the mother-infant dyad: A review. Birth Family J 4:12, 1977

7. de Chateau P: Parent-neonate interaction and its long term effects, in Simmel EG (ed): Early Experience and Early Behavior. New York, Academic Press, 1980

8. Lamb ME, Hwang C-P: Maternal attachment and mother-neonate bonding: A critical review, in Lamb ME, Brown AL

(eds): Advances in Developmental Psychology. Hilisdale,

NJ, Eribaum, 1982, vol 2

9. Hales DJ: Maternal behavior and hospital policy: An ecolog-ical experiment. Proceedings of the Society for Research in Child Development, New Orleans, April 1977

10. Hales DJ, Lozoff B, Sosa R, et al: Defining the limits of the maternal sensitive period. Dev Med Child Neurol 19:454,

1977

1 1. de Chateau P, Wiberg B: Long-term effect on mother-infant behavior of extra contact during the first hour post partum. I. First observations at 36 hours. Acta Paediatr Scand

66:137, 1977

12. de Chateau P, Wiberg B: Long-term effect on mother-infant behaviour of extra contact during the first hour post-partum. II. Follow-up at three months. Acta Paediatr Scand 66:145,

1977

13. de Chateau P: The influence of early contact on maternal

and infant behavior in prirniparae. Birth Family J 3:149,

1976

14. Hopkins JB: The Effect of Early and Extended Neonatal Contact on Mother-infant interaction, thesis. George Pea-body College, Nashville, TN, 1976

15. Hopkins JB, Vietze PM: Postpartum early and extended

contact: Quality, quantity or both? Proceedings of the

So-ciety for Research in Child Development, New Orleans,

April 1977

16. Grossman K, Thane K, Grossman KE: Maternal tactual

contact of the newborn after various postpartum conditions of mother-infant contact. Dev Psychol 17:159, 1981

17. Carlsson SG, Fagerberg H, Horneman G, et al: Effects of

various amounts of contact between mother and child on the mother’s nursing behavior. Dev Psychobiol 1 1:143, 1978

18. Schaller J, CarLsson SG, Larsson K: Effects ofextended

post-partum mother-child contact on the mother’s behavior dur-ing nursdur-ing. InfBehav Dec 2:319, 1979

19. Hwang C-P: Aspects of the mother-infant relationship dur-ing nursdur-ing, 1 and 6 weeks after early extended post-partum contact. Early Hum Dev 5:1, 1981

(6)

contact on early maternal attitudes, perceptions, and behav-iors. Proceedings of the Society for Research in Child

De-velopment, San Francisco, April 1979

21. Svejda MJ, Campos JJ, Emde RN: Mother-infant

“bonding”: Failure to generalize. Child Dev 51:775, 1980

22. Curry MAH: Contact during the first hour with the wrapped or naked newborn: Effect on maternal attachment behaviors at 36 hours and three months. Birth Family J 6:227, 1979 23. Anisfeld E, Upper E: Effects of perinatal events on

mother-infant bonding. Proceedings of the Society for Research in

Child Development, Boston, April 1981

24. Kennell JH, Jerauld R, Wolfe H, et al: Maternal behavior one year after early and extended post-parturn contact. Dev Med Child Neurol 16:172, 1974

25. Ringler NM, Kennell JH, Jarvella R, et al: Mother-to-child speech at 2 years-Effects of early postnatal contact. Behav Pediatr 86:141, 1975

26. Ringler NM, Trause MA, Klaus MH: Mother’s speech to her two-year-old: Its effect on speech and language comprehen-sion at 5 years. Pediatr Res 10:307, 1976

27. Rmgler N, Trause MA, Klaus MH, et a!: The effects of early post-partum contact and maternal speech patterns on chil-then’s IQ’s, speech, and language comprehension at five.

Child Dev 49:862, 1978

28. de Chateau P, Wiberg B: Long-term effect on mother-infant behavior of extra contact during the first hour post-partum.

III. One year follow-up. Dev Med Child Neurol, in press 1982

29. de Chateau P, Frankenhaeusei M, Lundberg M, et al: Three-year follow-up of early post-partum skin-to-skin contact. Proceedings of the Society for Research in Child Develop.

ment, Boston, April 1981

30. de Chateau P: Early post-partum contact and later attitudes.

mt

Behav Dev 3:273, 1980

31. Carlsson SG, Fagerberg H, Horneman G, et al: Effects of

various amounts of contact between mother and child on the mother’s nursing behavior: A follow-up study. Inf Behav Dev 2:209, 1979

32. Au

z,

Lo M: Early maternal-child contact: Effects on later behaviour. Dev Med Child Neurol 23:337, 1981 33. Siegel E, Bauman KE, Schaefer ES, et al: Hospital and home

support during infancy: Impact on maternal attachment, child abuse and neglect, and health care utilization. Pediat-rics 66:183, 1980

34. O’Connor S, Sherrod KB, Sandler MM, et a): The effect of extended post-partum contact on problems with parenting:

A controlled study of 301 families. Birth Family J 5:231,

1978

35. O’Connor S, Vietze P, Sherrod K, et al: How does

rooming-in enhance the mother-infant bond? Proceedings of the Society for Pediatric Research, June 1979

36. Vietze PM, O’Connor SM, Falsey 5, et al: Effects of

rooming-in and parity on maternal attachment behavior. Proceedings

of the American Psychological Association, Toronto,

Au-gust 1981

37. Ottaviano C, Campbell SBG, Taylor PM: Early contact and infant-mother attachment at one year. Proceedings of the Society for Research in Child Development, San Francisco,

April 1979

38. Campbell SBG, Maloni J, Dickey D: Early contact and maternal perceptions of infant temperament. Proceedings of

the Society for Research in Child Development, San Fran-cisco, April 1979

39. Grossman KE, Grossman K, Huber F, et al: German

chil-dren’s behavior towards their mothers at 12 months and

their fathers at 18 months in Ainsworth’s strange situation.

mt

J Behav Dev 4:157, 1981

40. Rode SS, Chang PN, Fisch RO, et al: Attachment patterns of infants separated at birth. Dec Psychol 17:188, 1981

41. Kontos D: A study of the effects of extended mother-infant contact on maternal behavior at one and three months.

Birth Family J 5:133, 1978

42. Sosa R, Klaus M, Kennell JH, et al: The effect of early mother-infant contact on breastfeeding, infection and

growth, in Breastfeeding and the Mother. Amsterdam,

El-sevier, 1976

43. Sousa PLR, Barros RC, Gazelle RV, et al: Attachment and lactation. Proceedings of the international Congress of

Pe-diatrics, Buenos Aires, 1974

44. Johnson NW: Breastfeeding at one hour of age. Am J

Maternal Child Nurs 1:12, 1976

45. de Chateau P, Holmberg H, Jakobson K, et al: A study of

factors promoting and inhibiting lactation. Dev Med Child

Neurol 19:575, 1977

46. Leiderman PH: The critical period hypothesis revisited: Mother to infant social bonding in the neonatal period, in Horowitz FD (ed): Early Developmental Hazards: Predic-tions and Precautions. Boulder, CO, Westview Press, 1978

47. Lozoff B: The sensitive period: An anthropological view. Proceedings of the Society for Research in Child

(7)

1982;70;763

Pediatrics

Michael E. Lamb

Early Contact and Maternal-Infant Bonding: One Decade Later

Services

Updated Information &

http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/70/5/763

including high resolution figures, can be found at:

Permissions & Licensing

http://www.aappublications.org/site/misc/Permissions.xhtml

entirety can be found online at:

Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures, tables) or in its

Reprints

http://www.aappublications.org/site/misc/reprints.xhtml

(8)

1982;70;763

Pediatrics

Michael E. Lamb

Early Contact and Maternal-Infant Bonding: One Decade Later

http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/70/5/763

the World Wide Web at:

The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is located on

American Academy of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 1073-0397.

References

Related documents

Given the high mutation rate and the importance of KRAS and p53 signaling pathways in regulating cell proliferation and survival, a genetically engineered mouse model of

77 Key , supra note 70 (“ Article 171 of the UNHCR Handbook speaks of the need for international condemnation for ‘the type of military action’ which the individual finds

Using titles such as Mathematics 40S (i.e., grade 12) Provincial Examination contrib- utes to the misconception that these tests evaluate knowledge learned in a particular

actually achieves genuine sexual and emotional intimacy with a partner, the experience defies physical definition: “When I grasped her [Jean Morris] it was like

On the basis of above analyses and results, it can be concluded that a positive and significant correlation exists between deceptive beauty advertising and the buying behavior

However, loss function of the unique components of TOR Complex II, such as Bit61, Avo2 or Avo3 did not affect the FBPase degradation in this study (Fig. 2B).Therefore,

In the procedure follows a simple and easy way to classify a given data set through a certain number of clusters (assume k clusters) fixed apriori. These centres should be

5 and 6 show the predicted and experimental effects of fiber volume fraction on the mechanical properties of the E-glass fiber chopped strand (random) mat