• No results found

Hair cortisol levels, perceived stress and body mass index in women and children living in socioeconomically disadvantaged neighborhoods: the READI study

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Hair cortisol levels, perceived stress and body mass index in women and children living in socioeconomically disadvantaged neighborhoods: the READI study"

Copied!
33
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

Olstad, Dana Lee, Ball, Kylie, Wright, Craig, Abbott, Gavin, Brown, Erin and Turner, Anne Isabella 2016, Hair cortisol levels, perceived stress and body mass index in women and children living in socioeconomically disadvantaged neighborhoods: the READI study, Stress, vol. 19, no. 2, pp. 158-167.

This is the accepted manuscript.

©2016, Informa UK

This is an Accepted Manuscript of an article published by Taylor & Francis in Stress, 29 March 2016, available at: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.3109/10253890.2016.1160282

Available from Deakin Research Online:

http://hdl.handle.net/10536/DRO/DU:30082730

(2)

1

Title: Hair cortisol levels, perceived stress and body mass index in women and children living in socioeconomically disadvantaged neighbourhoods: The READI study

Running head: Hair cortisol, perceived stress and BMI

Authors: Dana Lee Olstada*, Kylie Balla*, Craig Wrighta, Gavin Abbotta, Erin Browna, Anne

Isabella Turnera *Equal contributors

Affiliations: aCentre for Physical Activity and Nutrition Research, School of Exercise and Nutrition Sciences, Faculty of Health, Deakin University, 221 Burwood Highway, Burwood, VIC 3125 Australia.

Corresponding author: Professor Kylie Ball, Centre for Physical Activity and Nutrition Research, School of Exercise and Nutrition Sciences, Faculty of Health, Deakin University, 221 Burwood Highway, Burwood VIC 3125 Australia. Tel: +61 3 92517310; Email:

[email protected].

Email addresses: [email protected]; [email protected];

[email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected];

[email protected].

Keywords: perceived psychological stress; physiological stress; chronic stress; acute stress; obesity; BMI; socioeconomic disadvantage

(3)

2 ABSTRACT

Disadvantaged communities provide adverse psychosocial exposures that have been linked to high levels of stress, and this may provide one explanatory pathway linking socioeconomic disadvantage to obesity. This study used hair cortisol analysis to quantify associations between stress and body mass index (BMI), and between hair cortisol and perceived psychological stress levels, in women and children living in socioeconomically

disadvantaged neighbourhoods. Participants were a volunteer sample of 70 women from the Resilience for Eating and Activity Despite Inequality (READI) study, including 30 maternal-child pairs. Women self-reported body weight, height and perceived psychological stress using the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS), and provided hair samples for themselves and their child. Children’s body weight and height were measured. Following extraction, hair cortisol levels were measured using enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay. Multiple linear regression models examined associations between stress and BMI, and between hair cortisol and

perceived stress levels in women and children. Women's hair cortisol levels were not associated with their BMI or PSS scores. Women’s PSS scores were positively associated with their BMI (p=0.015). Within maternal-child pairs, mothers and children’s hair cortisol levels were strongly positively associated (p=0.006). Maternal hair cortisol levels and PSS scores were unrelated to their child's zBMI. Children's hair cortisol levels were not

associated with their zBMI or with their mother's PSS score. Findings suggest that cortisol-based and perceived psychological measures of stress may be distinct among women and children living in disadvantaged neighbourhoods. Perceived psychological measures may be more important predictors of weight-related risk.

(4)

3 INTRODUCTION

Obesity is among the most pressing public health issues worldwide due to its high prevalence and association with negative physical, psychological and social consequences across the lifecourse (Lim et al., 2012). A strong socioeconomic gradient in obesity exists in developed nations, such that those who are less educated, earn less income, live in disadvantaged

neighbourhoods, or are employed in less prestigious occupations face an increased risk (Ellaway et al., 1997; King et al., 2006; Matheson et al., 2008; Robert and Reither, 2004; van Lenthe and Mackenbach, 2002). In Australia for instance, the prevalence of overweight and obesity in 2011-12 among those living in the least disadvantaged areas was 50%, compared with 59% among those living in the most disadvantaged areas (Australian Bureau of

Statistics, 2013). It has been predicted that if current Australian trends do not abate, inequalities in weight will widen by 2025, such that there will be a 14 percentage point difference in obesity rates between the most (i.e. tertiary) and least (i.e. secondary only) educated Australian adults (Backholer et al., 2012).

The etiology of obesity is complex, as weight gain is influenced by factors ranging from inherited biological traits to behavioural, social and environmental factors; only some of which operate through their impact on dietary and physical activity behaviours (Government Office for Science, 2007). Accumulating evidence suggests psychosocial stress may be implicated in weight gain through direct biological and behavioural pathways (Moore and Cunningham, 2012; Wardle et al., 2011). Biologically, the stress response is characterised by activation of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis in an attempt to maintain

homeostasis (Bjorntorp, 2001). Activation of the HPA axis invokes increases in circulating cortisol, a glucocorticoid that increases abdominal adiposity and enhances appetite with

(5)

4

prolonged elevation (Bjorntorp, 2001). Behavioural mechanisms linking stress with obesity include increased consumption of energy-dense convenience foods (Tryon et al., 2015; Wardle J and Gibson EL, 2002), use of food for comfort or as a coping strategy (Dallman, 2010; Habhab et al., 2009; Zellner et al., 2006), and reduced participation in physical activity (Mouchacca et al., 2013; Steptoe et al., 1996). Together, these biological and behavioural responses establish a context tending towards weight gain. Children may be particularly susceptible to stress-induced weight gain, as both their own and their parents’ experiences of stress increase children’s risk of unhealthy dietary behaviours and weight gain (Gundersen et al., 2011; Gundersen et al., 2008; Parks et al., 2012; Shankardass et al., 2014). Moreover, evidence indicates that the level of cortisol in hair may be an inherited trait (Fairbanks et al., 2011).

Classic correlates of energy intake and expenditure do not fully explain socioeconomic gradients in obesity (Ball et al., 2002; Croft et al., 1992; Matheson et al., 2008; Pudrovska et al., 2014; Purslow et al., 2008). Given that socioeconomic disadvantage has been linked to high levels of psychosocial stress (Moore and Cunningham, 2012), elevated stress may provide one explanatory pathway linking socioeconomic disadvantage to obesity. Evidence indicates that individuals who reside in disadvantaged neighbourhoods are often embedded within environments in which they are relatively more likely to encounter negative or threatening situations (e.g. crime, disrepair, restricted access to grocery stores and

recreational amenities), leading to psychological distress and its associated biological and behavioural perturbations (Burdette and Hill, 2008; Hill et al., 2005; Ross et al., 2000). Moreover these studies show that neighbourhood disadvantage is an independent source of stress, over and above that conferred by individual-level SES and other factors. A large body of evidence indicates bi-directional associations among disadvantage, stress and obesity

(6)

5

(Burdette and Hill, 2008; Moore and Cunningham, 2012; Schulz et al., 2008), with a smaller number of studies suggesting that stress may mediate associations between disadvantage and obesity (Burdette and Hill, 2008; Moore and Cunningham, 2012; Schulz et al., 2008). Notably, this body of knowledge has been derived almost entirely from studies that have assessed perceived psychological stress levels, with consequent variation in ways in which the concept of stress has been defined and operationalized, and limitations associated with self-reporting. A smaller number of studies have examined these associations using physiological measures of stress such as salivary cortisol, finding that disadvantaged

neighbourhood contexts may influence baseline cortisol levels (Chen and Paterson, 2006) and patterns of cortisol secretion over the course of the day independent of individual-level SES (Do et al., 2011). A systematic review that examined associations between physiological measures of cortisol activity (primarily in saliva, urine and blood) and obesity found a general pattern wherein greater abdominal fat was associated with greater responsivity of the HPA axis, although some studies did show underresponsiveness (Incollingo Rodriguez et al., 2015). The analysis of physiological cortisol levels in saliva and blood has important

limitations, however, as cortisol levels in these mediums vary widely and systematically over the course of the day, reflect acute exposures up to minutes prior to data collection, are highly sensitive to context, and should ideally be sampled repeatedly over several days (Chan et al., 2014; Do et al., 2011; Fairbanks et al., 2011; Kudielka et al., 2009).

The limitations of extant methods of quantifying stress levels have led investigators to explore the feasibility of using hair cortisol analysis as an alternate or complementary

physiological marker of stress (Gow et al., 2010; Incollingo Rodriguez et al., 2015; Russell et al., 2012). The analysis of cortisol levels in hair provides a more objective marker of stress compared to psychological measures, and may be more reproducible than other physiological

(7)

6

measures of stress because it aggregates day-to-day variations in HPA axis activity, thereby reflecting chronic rather than acute and transient stressful exposures (Russell et al., 2012). Moreover, measurement is non-invasive and simple, requiring a single sample of hair. The validity of the technique is supported by studies showing that hair cortisol levels are elevated in patients with known hypercortisolism, and that they correspond with clinical disease course in patients with Cushing’s syndrome and Addison’s disease (Manenschijn et al., 2011; Thomson et al., 2010).

To date, hair cortisol analysis has not been used to examine associations between stress and obesity among individuals living in disadvantaged neighbourhoods who are at high risk of both. Different physiological measures of stress may show different associations with obesity compared to one another, and in comparison to perceived psychological measures. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to use hair cortisol analysis to quantify associations between a physiological indicator of chronic stress levels and body mass index (BMI), and between hair cortisol and perceived psychological stress levels in women and children living in socioeconomically disadvantaged neighbourhoods. We also considered the association between mother and child hair cortisol concentrations to examine the heritability of hair cortisol levels. We hypothesized that the hair cortisol and perceived stress levels of women living in disadvantaged areas would be positively associated with one another, and with their BMI. We expected that maternal and child hair cortisol levels would also show positive associations, and that children’s zBMI would be associated with their own hair cortisol levels and those of their mother.

MATERIALS AND METHODS Participants and procedures

(8)

7

Data for these analyses were provided in 2012-2013 by women and children participating in the Resilience for Eating and Activity Despite Inequality (READI) study, methods of which have been detailed elsewhere (Ball et al., 2013). The study was approved by the Deakin University Human Research Ethics Committee (HEAG-H 91_2006). Women provided written informed consent for themselves and their child to participate.

To obtain a representative sample of disadvantaged participants, READI used the Socio-Economic Index for Areas (SEIFA) (Australian Bureau of Statistics, 2001) to classify all neighbourhoods (suburbs) in Victoria, Australia, into tertiles of disadvantage. The SEIFA is based on 2001 population census data, and uses a broad definition of socioeconomic

disadvantage that reflects individuals’ access to material and social resources, and their ability to participate in society. Areas in the lowest SEIFA tertile were considered

disadvantaged, and 40 urban and 40 rural areas were randomly selected from this sampling frame. The electoral roll (voting is compulsory in Australia) was then used to randomly identify 150 women aged 18–45 years from each of these 80 areas (n=11 940; as some

included areas had fewer than 150 eligible women). Participants (n=4934; 41% response rate) replied to a postal invitation to complete a questionnaire (baseline; 2007-08). Those who consented to further follow-up and remained eligible to participate in the study (i.e. had not moved outside of the study neighbourhoods) were re-contacted to complete follow-up surveys 3 (2010-11) and 5 years (2012-13) after baseline. In 2012-13, completed surveys were received from 1560 women, of whom 277 also completed a child survey. A random sample of 199 of these women plus all 266 mother-child pairs who had consented to additional follow-up were then invited to take part in a sub-study involving hair cortisol analysis. A total of 116 women consented to take part in the sub-study, 60 of whom also consented on behalf of their child. Women who were pregnant were excluded.

(9)

8 Data collection and analysis

Perceived stress

Women’s perceived stress levels were assessed using the four-item version of the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS), a commonly used and validated measure of the degree to which

respondents felt their lives were unpredictable, uncontrollable or overwhelming in the past month (Cohen and Williamson, 1988; Cohen et al., 1983). The specific questions asked were: “During the last month how often have you: (i) felt that you were unable to control the important things in your life?; (ii) felt confident about your ability to handle your personal problems?; (iii) felt that things were going your way?; and (iv) felt difficulties were piling up so high that you could not overcome them?”. Responses options were: never, almost never, sometimes, fairly often, and very often. PSS scores were obtained by reverse coding the positive items (ii & iii), and summing scores for the four items. PSS scores can range from 0 – 16 (MacArthur Foundation), with an overall mean of 4.49 (SD 2.96) in a representative US sample (Cohen and Williamson, 1988). Higher scores indicated greater perceived stress.

Body mass index

Women self-reported height and weight to calculate BMI (kg/m2). Children’s height (to the nearest 0.1 cm) and weight (to the nearest 0.1 kg) were measured without shoes and in light clothing by trained research assistants within 4 months of maternal survey completion using a portable stadiometer and digital scales. Age- and sex-adjusted BMI z-scores (zBMI) were calculated for children based on the Centers for Disease Control reference population (Kuczmarski et al., 2000). Children were classified as normal weight, overweight or obese using cut-off points established by Cole et al (Cole et al., 2000).

(10)

9 Sociodemographic factors

Women reported their age, country of birth, smoking status (never, former, current

occasional, current regular), highest education level (low: did not complete year 12, medium: completed year 12 or equivalent, high: tertiary qualification), marital status (married/de facto, previously married, never married), number of children living in the home (0, 1, 2, ≥ 3), employment status (working full time, working part-time, not currently in paid employment), and household income (low: $0–699 per week, medium: $700–1499 per week, high: $1500+ per week, undisclosed), and whether they had dyed their hair (yes, no).

Hair samples

Hair sample collection: Consenting women were sent a hair sampling kit. The kit contained instructions on how to cut approximately 100 strands of hair as close to the scalp as possible from the posterior vertex position of the head for themselves and their child. This area of the scalp was selected because it has the lowest coefficient of variation (15.6%) compared to hair sampled from other areas (30.5%) (Sauve et al., 2007). Women then taped the hair sample to a paper slip provided with the scalp end marked. Samples were returned in sealed postage-paid envelopes, as per standard procedures (Gow et al., 2010).

A total of 102 hair samples were received from women (22% of those invited to participate) and 54 from children (20% of those invited to participate). Excluding participants whose hair samples were unusable (too few or too short strands, or did not mark the scalp end), samples from 74 women and 40 children were received within 3-4 months of survey completion. This timing was suitable, since 3 cm of hair were used in the analysis, representing approximately 3 months of stress exposure, as hair grows at an average of 1 cm per month (Wennig, 2000). Hair samples were stored at room temperature for up to 12 months prior to analysis.

(11)

10

Hair sample preparation and cortisol extraction: Hair samples were removed from the tape and attached to a Hoffman screw compressor clamp (Science Supply Australia, Vic, AU). Each hair segment was washed thoroughly in isopropanol and dried at room temperature overnight. The most proximal 3 cm of hair to the scalp (where available) was divided into three 1 cm segments and finely chopped into separate glass vials using surgical scissors. One or two 1 cm segments were used where this was all that was available. Samples were

weighed (~15-20 mg per sample) and 1 ml of methanol was added to each sample. The vials were sealed and incubated on an orbital shaker at 150 RPM for 16 hours at 52°C. Methanol was transferred to a glass Kimble tube and evaporated under a nitrogen gas stream at 50°C. The remaining residue in the tube was reconstituted in 250 µl of phosphate buffered saline (pH 8.0) and vortexed for 60 seconds. These methods for cortisol extraction from hair

samples were based on those used by Manenschijn et al (Manenschijn et al., 2011) and Sauve et al (Sauve et al., 2007).

Hair cortisol analysis:

Cortisol levels were measured using an Enzyme-Linked Immunosorbent Assay for salivary cortisol (Salivary ELISA Cortisol kit©; Alpco Diagnostics, Windham, NH) as per the manufacturer’s protocol. The use of salivary cortisol ELISA to assess cortisol levels in hair has been shown to yield valid and reliable results (Gow et al., 2010; Manenschijn et al., 2011; Russell et al., 2012; Thomson et al., 2010). The intra-assay coefficient of variation was 3.96 % and the inter-assay coefficient of variation was 7.75 %. The inter-assay coefficient of variation was determined by using an internal control sample which was run on each plate. As there was no statistical variation in the cortisol levels among the three 1 cm segments provided by each participant, the analysis was performed on the average cortisol levels across

(12)

11

the three hair segments where available, otherwise one or two segments were used. Three hair segments were available for 93% of women and 83% of children (Table 1). This method for hair cortisol analysis was based on methods used by Manenschijn et al (Manenschijn et al., 2011) and Sauve et al (Sauve et al., 2007).

Statistical analyses

Among the 74 women with hair cortisol data, three had extreme outlier values (> 3SD above the mean) for average cortisol level and were excluded, leaving 71 women with hair cortisol data. One of the women excluded was part of a maternal-child pair, therefore data for her child, who also had outlier values, were excluded. Of the remaining 71 women with hair cortisol data, 37 also had hair cortisol data for their child. Three children had extreme outlier (high) values for cortisol and were excluded, leaving 34 maternal-child pairs with hair

cortisol data. Participants with missing BMI data were also excluded (n=1 woman, n=4 children); thus data from 70 women and 30 maternal-child pairs were included in the final analysis.

Cortisol data for both women and children were positively skewed, and therefore these variables were log transformed for use in associational analyses. Linear regression models with robust standard errors were used to test bivariate associations between each of the following: 1) women’s hair cortisol level and their BMI, PSS score, their child’s zBMI and hair cortisol level; 2) women’s PSS score and their BMI, their child’s zBMI and hair cortisol level; and 3) children’s hair cortisol level and their zBMI.

Crude and adjusted models for women and children were calculated. For women, education (low, medium, high), smoking status (current, never/former) and the number of children

(13)

12

living in the home (0, ≥ 1) were included in adjusted models, while for children, maternal BMI (continuous), maternal education (low, medium, high) and child sex were included. Associations with a variety of other potential confounders (age, country of birth, marital status, household income, employment status, hair dying) were tested but were not statistically significant at p<0.10 and hence these variables were not controlled for in analyses.

RESULTS

Characteristics of the women and children included in the analyses are presented in Table 1. Women were on average 43 (± 7.2 SD) years of age. The majority of women were born in Australia and married. Nearly 43% worked full-time, 41% had a tertiary education and 46% reported a household income ≥ $1500 per week. One-third of women were overweight, while one-fifth were obese. The mean hair cortisol level for women was 123.7 ng/g (SD=71.2 ng/g), while PSS scores averaged 5.4 (SD=2.6). Children were 14 years of age on average. Twenty percent of children were overweight and 10% were obese. Children’s hair cortisol levels averaged 96.6 ng/g (SD=49.6 ng/g).

Scatterplots of women’s hair cortisol levels and their BMI (Figure 1A), women’s PSS scores and their BMI (Figure 1B), and maternal and child hair cortisol levels (Figure 1C) are

presented as Figures. Women's hair cortisol levels were not associated with their BMI or PSS scores (Table 2). By contrast, women’s PSS scores were positively associated with their BMI in both crude (β=0.71 [95% CI=0.14, 1.28], p=0.015) and adjusted (β=0.65 [95% CI=0.06, 1.23], p=0.032) models. Within maternal-child pairs, mothers and children’s hair cortisol levels were strongly positively associated (crude: β=0.49 [95% CI=0.15, 0.83], p=0.006; adjusted: β=0.51 [95% CI=0.17, 0.84], p=0.004). Maternal hair cortisol levels and PSS

(14)

13

scores were unrelated to their child's zBMI (Table 2). Children's hair cortisol levels were not associated with their zBMI or with their mother's PSS score (Table 2).

DISCUSSION

Chronic stress represents a bio-behavioural mechanism through which contemporary lifestyles may promote obesogenic behaviours and body fat accumulation. This study examined associations between hair cortisol levels, perceived stress and BMI among women and children living in disadvantaged areas. Findings showed that women’s BMI was

positively associated with perceived psychological, but not with a physiological measure of stress, whereas children’s BMI was unrelated to any maternal or child stress measures. There was also no association between women’s perceived stress and their own, or their child’s (in the case of maternal-child pairs) hair cortisol levels. Mother and child hair cortisol levels were, however, strongly positively associated.

The assertion that psychological distress arising from neighbourhood-level disadvantage can increase the risk of obesity derives primarily from cross-sectional studies linking

neighbourhood disadvantage with perceived stress (Feldman and Steptoe, 2004; Hill et al., 2005; Ross et al., 2000; Steptoe and Feldman, 2001) or obesity (Ellaway et al., 1997; King et al., 2006; Matheson et al., 2008; Moore and Cunningham, 2012; Robert and Reither, 2004; van Lenthe and Mackenbach, 2002) on the one hand, and connecting perceived stress with obesity on the other (Moore and Cunningham, 2012; Mouchacca et al., 2013; Wardle et al., 2011). Our findings that the perceived psychological stress levels of women residing in disadvantaged neighbourhoods were associated with their BMI are therefore consistent with this body of knowledge. Moreover, as in other studies (Burdette and Hill, 2008; Hill et al., 2005; Matheson et al., 2008), these associations persisted after adjustment for

(15)

individual-14

level measures of SES. Although we were unable to conduct a mediation analysis due to the small sample size, evidence from two other studies suggests that psychological stress may mediate associations between neighbourhood disadvantage and obesity. In the first, psychological distress mediated positive associations between perceived neighbourhood disorder (an index of perceived crime, noise and cleanliness) and obesity risk among a probability sample of 1338 American adults (Burdette and Hill, 2008). In the second, associations among neighbourhood material deprivation, gender and BMI were reduced by 8% when perceived stress and lifestyle and health-related behaviours were accounted for in a representative sample of 64,277 Canadian adults (Matheson et al., 2008). These associations were independent of individual-level measures of SES.

While women’s perceived psychological stress levels were associated with their BMI in the present study, their physiological hair cortisol levels were not. Several studies in other adult populations have similarly found no relationship between hair cortisol levels and BMI (Boesch et al., 2015; Manenschijn et al., 2011; Manenschijn et al., 2013), whereas others have reported small positive associations (Manenschijn et al., 2011; Stalder et al., 2012; Stalder et al., 2013). Manenschijn et al (Manenschijn et al., 2011) reported that although hair cortisol levels were not correlated with BMI in healthy individuals, they were positively correlated with waist circumference. Given that long-term exposure to elevated cortisol levels leads to accumulation of visceral fat (Bjorntorp and Rosmond, 2000), and that BMI is indicative of general and not necessarily abdominal obesity, it is possible that waist

circumference, which was not measured in our study, may have proved a more sensitive marker of the exposures we sought to investigate. This conclusion is supported by a recent systematic review which found that abdominal adiposity was associated with increased HPA axis activity, whereas associations with overall adiposity (i.e. BMI) were more mixed

(16)

15

(Incollingo Rodriguez et al., 2015). These findings may reflect tissue-specific alterations in cortisol metabolism (Incollingo Rodriguez et al., 2015; Rask et al., 2001). Thus, future studies should examine stress in relation to both BMI and waist circumference, as individuals with high BMI but low abdominal adiposity may exhibit different cortisol profiles compared to those with low BMI and high abdominal adiposity (Incollingo Rodriguez et al., 2015).

Previous studies have found significantly higher hair cortisol levels in obese compared to non-obese patients, but no difference in hair cortisol levels between normal and overweight (non-obese) individuals (Manenschijn et al., 2011; Wester et al., 2014). If cortisol levels only become elevated at BMI levels within the obese range, it is plausible that the lack of

association between hair cortisol levels and BMI in the current study might alternatively relate to the small number of women who self-reported a BMI in the obese range (n=13). Given the well-known tendency of women to underreport BMI (Merrill and Richardson, 2009), the current findings may be biased towards the null and may have differed had we measured women’s BMI. However, self-reported BMI has been shown to correspond with objective measures in Australian women (Burton et al., 2010). Differences among studies may also partially relate to the use of categorical versus continuous measures of BMI.

The stress response in humans is characterized by multiple physiological and psychological changes. Two of the most prominent responses involve activation of the HPA axis and negative subjective psychological states such as nervousness and tension (Schlotz et al., 2008). Measures of one or both of these response systems can be used to gauge stress levels (Schlotz et al., 2008). Theoretically, physiological and psychological measures of stress should be associated because they presumably represent indicators of the same construct (Schlotz et al., 2008); however in practice this is often not the case (Boesch et al., 2015; Chan

(17)

16

et al., 2014; Dettenborn et al., 2010; Dowlati et al., 2010; Karlen et al., 2011; Stalder et al., 2012; Van Uum et al., 2008). Similarly in the current study, women’s perceived stress levels were unrelated to their hair cortisol levels. It is possible (Schlotz et al., 2008) that these inconsistencies may reflect different dynamics of the two systems; as whereas subjective psychological responses occur rapidly and change dynamically, physiological HPA axis responses have a slower onset and rate of change (Schlotz et al., 2008). This lag in short-term physiological endocrine behind psychological stress responses is termed a “lack of

psychoendocrine covariance,” and has led to speculation that subjective psychological states may mediate associations between stressors and endocrine stress responses (Schlotz et al., 2008). Psychoendocrine cross-correlations may change over time in response to various stressors (Schlotz et al., 2008), however, and therefore it is unclear whether and to what extent findings from short-term studies may be invoked to explain the longer-term stress responses assessed in the current study.

Others have suggested that physiological and psychological stress levels may in fact represent distinct constructs, such that hair cortisol levels reflect a qualitatively different dimension of stress-related exposures than is captured by individuals’ subjective experiences (Stalder et al., 2012). A number of differences between these measures support this notion. For instance, it seems unlikely that a brief four-item questionnaire can capture participants’ experiences of stress across multiple life domains to the same extent as a physiological measure.

Alternatively, it might also be said that hair cortisol analysis is limited to providing an indication of overall HPA axis activity over a defined period. In addition, hair cortisol measures are insensitive to brief stressors such as daily hassles that may temporarily elevate PSS scores (Russell et al., 2012). Alternatively, cConsistent exposure to stress could lead to habituation (Nguyen and Peschard K, 2003), attenuating stress-related appraisals relative to

(18)

17

physiological measures. Notably however, the cortisol response can habituate as well, as initial hypercortisolism has been shown to subside into hypocortisolism with advancing chronicity of stress (Miller et al., 2007). Moreover, emerging evidence indicates that the experience of living in a disadvantaged neighbourhood that one perceives as stressful is associated with hypocortisolism, rather than hypercortisolism (Karb et al., 2012). Future studies comparing the response to stressors across these two systems are essential to clarify the distinctions between psychological and physiological stress responses. Ongoing

measurement of both systems appears advisable, given that they may provide complementary information. For instance, perceived psychological measures can augment information gleaned from physiological measures by providing information regarding the source and types of stressors. Diurnal patterns of cortisol activity may also help to explain

socioeconomic differentials (Do et al., 2011; Karb et al., 2012; Van Ryzin et al., 2009), and therefore concurrent collection of saliva samples may yield additional insights.

The negative impacts of stress may be particularly damaging to children, as chronic alterations in cortisol secretion could influence development of brain, endocrine and metabolic systems (Hemmingsson, 2014). In light of this plasticity it has been posited that the origins of socioeconomic status-health connections can be traced to stressful conditions experienced by children, particularly those from socioeconomically disadvantaged

backgrounds (Finkelstein et al., 2007; Matthews et al., 2010). In addition to environmental influences, genetic heritability or maternal calibration of the child’s HPA axis may also be implicated (Karlen et al., 2013), as studies indicate that high levels of cortisol in hair may be an inherited trait (Fairbanks et al., 2011). In the current study, mother’s and children’s hair cortisol levels were strongly associated, perhaps reflecting shared environmental or genetic influences, or both. We found no evidence that the potentially negative body weight-related

(19)

18

impacts of maternal stressors were transmitted intergenerationally, however, as perceived measures of stress and hair cortisol levels were not associated with measured BMI in children. These findings differ from those of a systematic review that found consistent evidence of associations between reported experiences of stress at the household level and childhood obesity (Gundersen et al., 2011), and subsequent studies finding that psychological stress experienced by parents contributed to obesity risk in children of varying ages and income levels both cross-sectionally (Koch et al., 2008; Parks et al., 2012; Shankardass et al., 2014) and longitudinally (Koch et al., 2008).

Children’s BMI was also unrelated to their own hair cortisol levels. This finding is at odds with evidence indicating that obese children have higher hair cortisol concentrations

compared to their non-obese counterparts (Veldhorst et al., 2014). However, our results are consistent with a study which found no relationship between children’s BMI and their salivary cortisol levels (Vanaelst et al., 2014). Given the paucity of studies that have assessed hair cortisol in relation to BMI in children, we can only speculate that the lack of associations observed may reflect the small number of children who were obese in our sample, a lack of measurements of waist circumference to indicate visceral obesity [46], or perhaps the wide variability in their age, as different stressors may influence BMI among younger and older children (Garasky et al., 2009). Children’s body weight is determined by a host of factors originating as far back as the pre-pregnancy period (Barouki et al., 2012; Heindel and Vandenberg, 2015) and it may therefore be the case that other factors were simply stronger determinants of BMI among children in the current sample. Children’s perceived stress levels have been associated with overweight and obesity in other studies (Gundersen et al., 2011; Vanaelst et al., 2014), suggesting that perceived psychological measures of stress may also be more important predictors of weight-related risk among

(20)

19

children as was the case among women in the current study. Nevertheless, we did not measure perceived stress levels among children. Alternatively, the general lack of

associations observed between hair cortisol levels and BMI in both women and children in the current study may indicate that the stress levels captured by hair cortisol measures used were not representative of longer-term exposures that may predispose to unhealthy weight gain. Additional studies evaluating associations between perceived psychological and physiological measures of stress and adiposity in larger samples of women and children are essential to clarify these relationships.

Strengths and limitations

As this is the first study to examine hair cortisol levels in relation to BMI among disadvantaged women and children, we chose to begin by examining cross-sectional

relationships. However, given that body weight is determined over the course of many years, and that stress levels in the past 1-3 months may not reflect longer-term stress levels,

longitudinal studies that assess change in BMI in relation to change in stress levels are ultimately needed to understand these relationships. The women in our sample lived in disadvantaged neighbourhoods, however many were relatively advantaged on average according to individual-level measures of SES. It is therefore possible that some women’s greater access to individual-level resources mitigated some of the detrimental impacts of their neighbourhood contexts, potentially masking associations. This might explain why women’s average PSS scores (mean 5.4, SD 2.6) were not markedly elevated compared to those

observed in a representative US sample (mean 4.49, SD 2.96) (Cohen and Williamson, 1988). A large number of studies have, however, shown that neighbourhood disadvantage is

(21)

20

SES (Burdette and Hill, 2008; Do et al., 2011; Hill et al., 2005; Karb et al., 2012; Matheson et al., 2008).

Because the hypothesized causal pathways were observed simultaneously, it is not possible to determine the direction of causation in relationships between women’s PSS scores and BMI. This study focused on the relationship between stress and BMI, although many other

individual, social and environmental factors may also influence BMI. The exclusion of men, the small sample size, and low response rates are further limitations that may limit

generalisability of our findings. It is possible that the women and children excluded due to extremely high hair cortisol values may have been using corticosteroid medications, however we are unable to verify this as we did not collect information about medications such as corticosteroid creams. Future studies could compare hair cortisol levels and corresponding associations with BMI between advantaged and disadvantaged groups given that there is no established range of hair cortisol levels (Russell et al., 2012), and a wide range of values have been reported in other studies (Chan et al., 2014; Raul et al., 2004; Sauve et al., 2007; Stalder et al., 2012; Wester et al., 2014). Finally, it is possible that the different time frames captured

by our measures (1 month for PSS and 3 months for hair cortisol) could partially account for

lack of associations between PSS and hair cortisol levels. However, it appears more likely that it is not the time frame over which stressors were measured that was influential, but that these two measures may capture qualitatively different dimensions of stress.

Despite these limitations this study provides an important platform for future inquiry in this area, and has a number of important strengths. First, we used a physiological measure of chronic stress to complement a perceived psychological measure of stress, integrating both environmental and biological mechanisms to explain weight-related disparities. Second, BMI

(22)

21

was measured in children, although it was based on self-report among women. Finally, although the sample size was small, due to difficulties in finding large numbers of women and children willing to provide hair samples, it was consistent with the sample sizes of similar studies (Chan et al., 2014; Dettenborn et al., 2010; Kalra et al., 2007; Stalder et al., 2012).

CONCLUSIONS

Associations between disadvantage and obesity cannot be fully explained by classic

correlates of energy intake and expenditure (Ball et al., 2003; Martikainen and Marmot, 1999; Purslow et al., 2008). It has therefore been hypothesized that the higher levels of stress experienced byindividuals living in disadvantaged neighbourhoods may partially explain disparities in body weight between those residing in advantaged and disadvantaged areas. Findings from this study showed that women’s BMI was associated with perceived

psychological stress, but not with their physiological hair cortisol levels, whereas children’s BMI was unrelated to any maternal or child stress measures. Physiological and subjective psychological measures of stress may therefore be distinct, where psychological measures may be more important predictors of weight-related risk. Importantly, there was a strong, positive association between maternal and child physiological stress levels. These results provide a platform for future, larger studies that can help to elucidate the pathways linking stress and obesity in women and children living in disadvantaged neighbourhoods and potential intergenerational linkages.

Given the differences observed in associations between a perceived psychological and a physiological measure of stress with BMI, and the fact that hair cortisol analysis provides no information as to the source or types of stressors, future studies should integrate physiological

(23)

22

measures of stress with perceived psychological measures that discriminate between sources and types of stress. Such investigations will provide a more complete perspective of stress-related risk of weight gain, and are better able to inform targets for intervention. Finally if as our findings suggest, stressful conditions perceived by individuals living in disadvantaged neighbourhoods are truly associated with elevated BMI, policies and programs that aim to ameliorate these perceptions should be a high priority.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This study was funded by a National Health & Medical Research Council (NHMRC) Strategic Award, ID 374241. The study funder had no role in study design, collection, analysis or interpretation of the data, in writing the report, or in the decision to submit the manuscript for publication. The contents of this paper are the responsibility of the authors and do not reflect the views of the NHMRC. DLO is supported by a Canadian Institutes of Health Research Fellowship and an Endeavour Research Fellowship. KB is supported by a National Health & Medical Research Council Principal Research Fellowship, ID 1042442. DECLARATION OF INTEREST STATEMENT

(24)

23 REFERENCES

Australian Bureau of Statistics. Census of Population and Housing - SEIFA 2001 2001 [March 17, 2015]. Available from:

http://www.abs.gov.au/websitedbs/D3110124.NSF/f5c7b8fb229cf017ca256973001fecec/a17dc48d 988ecf63ca256dad00005ea3!OpenDocument.

Australian Bureau of Statistics. Persons who are overweight or obese by remoteness and Index of Relative Socio-Economic Disadvantage (Table 6) 2013 [April 20, 2015]. Available from:

http://www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/[email protected]/Lookup/33C64022ABB5ECD5CA257B8200179437?opend ocument.

Backholer K, Mannan HR, Magliano DJ, Walls HL, Stevenson C, Beauchamp A, Shaw JE, Peeters A. Projected socioeconomic disparities in the prevalence of obesity among Australian adults. Aust N Z J Public Health. 2012;36(6):557-63.

Ball K, Mishra G, Crawford D. Which aspects of socioeconomic status are related to obesity among men and women? International journal of obesity and related metabolic disorders : journal of the International Association for the Study of Obesity. 2002;26(4):559-65.

Ball K, Mishra GD, Crawford D. Social factors and obesity: an investigation of the role of health behaviours. International journal of obesity and related metabolic disorders : journal of the International Association for the Study of Obesity. 2003;27(3):394-403.

Ball K, Cleland V, Salmon J, Timperio AF, McNaughton S, Thornton L, Campbell K, Jackson M, Baur LA, Mishra G, Brug J, Jeffery RW, King A, Kawachi I, Crawford DA. Cohort profile: the resilience for eating and activity despite inequality (READI) study. International journal of epidemiology.

2013;42(6):1629-39.

Barouki R, Gluckman PD, Grandjean P, Hanson M, Heindel JJ. Developmental origins of non-communicable disease: implications for research and public health. Environ Health. 2012;11:42. Bjorntorp P. Do stress reactions cause abdominal obesity and comorbidities? Obesity reviews : an official journal of the International Association for the Study of Obesity. 2001;2(2):73-86.

Bjorntorp P, Rosmond R. Obesity and cortisol. Nutrition. 2000;16(10):924-36.

Boesch M, Sefidan S, Annen H, Ehlert U, Roos L, Van Uum S, Russell E, Koren G, La Marca R. Hair cortisol concentration is unaffected by basic military training, but related to sociodemographic and environmental factors. Stress. 2015;18(1):35-41.

Burdette AM, Hill TD. An examination of processes linking perceived neighborhood disorder and obesity. Social science & medicine. 2008;67(1):38-46.

Burton NW, Brown W, Dobson A. Accuracy of body mass index estimated from self-reported height and weight in mid-aged Australian women. Australian and New Zealand journal of public health. 2010;34(6):620-3.

Chan J, Sauve B, Tokmakejian S, Koren G, Van Uum S. Measurement of cortisol and testosterone in hair of obese and non-obese human subjects. Experimental and clinical endocrinology & diabetes : official journal, German Society of Endocrinology [and] German Diabetes Association.

2014;122(6):356-62.

Chen E, Paterson LQ. Neighborhood, family, and subjective socioeconomic status: How do they relate to adolescent health? Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association. 2006;25(6):704-14.

Cohen S, Williamson G. The social psychology of health. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; c1988. Perceived stress in a probability sample of the United States; p. 31-67.

Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of health and social behavior. 1983;24(4):385-96.

Cole TJ, Bellizzi MC, Flegal KM, Dietz WH. Establishing a standard definition for child overweight and obesity worldwide: international survey. BMJ. 2000;320(7244):1240-3.

(25)

24

Croft JB, Strogatz DS, James SA, Keenan NL, Ammerman AS, Malarcher AM, Haines PS.

Socioeconomic and behavioral correlates of body mass index in black adults: the Pitt County Study. American journal of public health. 1992;82(6):821-6.

Dallman MF. Stress-induced obesity and the emotional nervous system. Trends in endocrinology and metabolism: TEM. 2010;21(3):159-65.

Dettenborn L, Tietze A, Bruckner F, Kirschbaum C. Higher cortisol content in hair among long-term unemployed individuals compared to controls. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35(9):1404-9. Do DP, Diez Roux AV, Hajat A, Auchincloss AH, Merkin SS, Ranjit N, Shea S, Seeman T. Circadian rhythm of cortisol and neighborhood characteristics in a population-based sample: the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis. Health Place. 2011;17(2):625-32.

Dowlati Y, Herrmann N, Swardfager W, Thomson S, Oh PI, Van Uum S, Koren G, Lanctot KL. Relationship between hair cortisol concentrations and depressive symptoms in patients with coronary artery disease. Neuropsychiatric disease and treatment. 2010;6:393-400.

Ellaway A, Anderson A, Macintyre S. Does area of residence affect body size and shape? International journal of obesity and related metabolic disorders : journal of the International Association for the Study of Obesity. 1997;21(4):304-8.

Fairbanks LA, Jorgensen MJ, Bailey JN, Breidenthal SE, Grzywa R, Laudenslager ML. Heritability and genetic correlation of hair cortisol in vervet monkeys in low and higher stress environments. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2011;36(8):1201-8.

Feldman PJ, Steptoe A. How neighborhoods and physical functioning are related: the roles of neighborhood socioeconomic status, perceived neighborhood strain, and individual health risk factors. Annals of behavioral medicine : a publication of the Society of Behavioral Medicine. 2004;27(2):91-9.

Finkelstein DM, Kubzansky LD, Capitman J, Goodman E. Socioeconomic differences in adolescent stress: the role of psychological resources. The Journal of adolescent health : official publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine. 2007;40(2):127-34.

Garasky S, Stewart SD, Gundersen C, Lohman BJ, Eisenmann JC. Family stressors and child obesity. Social science research. 2009;38(4):755-66.

Government Office for Science. Foresight. Tackling obesities: Future choices - Project report 2007. Available from:

https://www.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/287937/07-1184x-tackling-obesities-future-choices-report.pdf.

Gow R, Thomson S, Rieder M, Van Uum S, Koren G. An assessment of cortisol analysis in hair and its clinical applications. Forensic science international. 2010;196(1-3):32-7.

Gundersen C, Mahatmya D, Garasky S, Lohman B. Linking psychosocial stressors and childhood obesity. Obesity reviews : an official journal of the International Association for the Study of Obesity. 2011;12(5):e54-63.

Gundersen C, Lohman BJ, Garasky S, Stewart S, Eisenmann J. Food security, maternal stressors, and overweight among low-income US children: results from the National Health and Nutrition

Examination Survey (1999-2002). Pediatrics. 2008;122(3):e529-40.

Habhab S, Sheldon JP, Loeb RC. The relationship between stress, dietary restraint, and food preferences in women. Appetite. 2009;52(2):437-44.

Heindel JJ, Vandenberg LN. Developmental origins of health and disease: a paradigm for understanding disease cause and prevention. Curr Opin Pediatr. 2015;27(2):248-53.

Hemmingsson E. A new model of the role of psychological and emotional distress in promoting obesity: conceptual review with implications for treatment and prevention. Obesity reviews : an official journal of the International Association for the Study of Obesity. 2014;15(9):769-79.

Hill TD, Ross CE, Angel RJ. Neighborhood disorder, psychophysiological distress, and health. Journal of health and social behavior. 2005;46(2):170-86.

(26)

25

Incollingo Rodriguez AC, Epel ES, White ML, Standen EC, Seckl JR, Tomiyama AJ. Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis dysregulation and cortisol activity in obesity: A systematic review. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2015;62:301-18.

Kalra S, Einarson A, Karaskov T, Van Uum S, Koren G. The relationship between stress and hair cortisol in healthy pregnant women. Clinical and investigative medicine Medecine clinique et experimentale. 2007;30(2):E103-7.

Karb RA, Elliott MR, Dowd JB, Morenoff JD. Neighborhood-level stressors, social support, and diurnal patterns of cortisol: the Chicago Community Adult Health Study. Social science & medicine.

2012;75(6):1038-47.

Karlen J, Ludvigsson J, Frostell A, Theodorsson E, Faresjo T. Cortisol in hair measured in young adults - a biomarker of major life stressors? BMC clinical pathology. 2011;11:12.

Karlen J, Frostell A, Theodorsson E, Faresjo T, Ludvigsson J. Maternal influence on child HPA axis: a prospective study of cortisol levels in hair. Pediatrics. 2013;132(5):e1333-40.

King T, Kavanagh AM, Jolley D, Turrell G, Crawford D. Weight and place: a multilevel cross-sectional survey of area-level social disadvantage and overweight/obesity in Australia. International journal of obesity. 2006;30(2):281-7.

Koch FS, Sepa A, Ludvigsson J. Psychological stress and obesity. The Journal of pediatrics. 2008;153(6):839-44.

Kuczmarski RJ, Ogden CL, Grummer-Strawn LM, Flegal KM, Guo SS, Wei R, Mei Z, Curtin LR, Roche AF, Johnson CL. CDC growth charts: United States. Advance data. 2000;(314):1-27.

Kudielka BM, Hellhammer DH, Wust S. Why do we respond so differently? Reviewing determinants of human salivary cortisol responses to challenge. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34(1):2-18. Lim SS, Vos T, Flaxman AD, Danaei G, Shibuya K, Adair-Rohani H, Amann M, Anderson HR, Andrews KG, Aryee M, Atkinson C, Bacchus LJ, Bahalim AN, Balakrishnan K, Balmes J, Barker-Collo S, Baxter A, Bell ML, Blore JD, Blyth F, Bonner C, Borges G, Bourne R, Boussinesq M, Brauer M, Brooks P, Bruce NG, Brunekreef B, Bryan-Hancock C, Bucello C, Buchbinder R, Bull F, Burnett RT, Byers TE, Calabria B, Carapetis J, Carnahan E, Chafe Z, Charlson F, Chen H, Chen JS, Cheng AT, Child JC, Cohen A, Colson KE, Cowie BC, Darby S, Darling S, Davis A, Degenhardt L, Dentener F, Des Jarlais DC, Devries K, Dherani M, Ding EL, Dorsey ER, Driscoll T, Edmond K, Ali SE, Engell RE, Erwin PJ, Fahimi S, Falder G, Farzadfar F, Ferrari A, Finucane MM, Flaxman S, Fowkes FG, Freedman G, Freeman MK, Gakidou E, Ghosh S, Giovannucci E, Gmel G, Graham K, Grainger R, Grant B, Gunnell D, Gutierrez HR, Hall W, Hoek HW, Hogan A, Hosgood HD, 3rd, Hoy D, Hu H, Hubbell BJ, Hutchings SJ, Ibeanusi SE, Jacklyn GL, Jasrasaria R, Jonas JB, Kan H, Kanis JA, Kassebaum N, Kawakami N, Khang YH, Khatibzadeh S, Khoo JP, Kok C, Laden F, Lalloo R, Lan Q, Lathlean T, Leasher JL, Leigh J, Li Y, Lin JK, Lipshultz SE, London S, Lozano R, Lu Y, Mak J, Malekzadeh R, Mallinger L, Marcenes W, March L, Marks R, Martin R, McGale P, McGrath J, Mehta S, Mensah GA, Merriman TR, Micha R, Michaud C, Mishra V, Mohd Hanafiah K, Mokdad AA, Morawska L, Mozaffarian D, Murphy T, Naghavi M, Neal B, Nelson PK, Nolla JM,

Norman R, Olives C, Omer SB, Orchard J, Osborne R, Ostro B, Page A, Pandey KD, Parry CD, Passmore E, Patra J, Pearce N, Pelizzari PM, Petzold M, Phillips MR, Pope D, Pope CA, 3rd, Powles J, Rao M, Razavi H, Rehfuess EA, Rehm JT, Ritz B, Rivara FP, Roberts T, Robinson C, Rodriguez-Portales JA, Romieu I, Room R, Rosenfeld LC, Roy A, Rushton L, Salomon JA, Sampson U, Sanchez-Riera L, Sanman E, Sapkota A, Seedat S, Shi P, Shield K, Shivakoti R, Singh GM, Sleet DA, Smith E, Smith KR, Stapelberg NJ, Steenland K, Stockl H, Stovner LJ, Straif K, Straney L, Thurston GD, Tran JH, Van Dingenen R, van Donkelaar A, Veerman JL, Vijayakumar L, Weintraub R, Weissman MM, White RA, Whiteford H, Wiersma ST, Wilkinson JD, Williams HC, Williams W, Wilson N, Woolf AD, Yip P, Zielinski JM, Lopez AD, Murray CJ, Ezzati M, AlMazroa MA, Memish ZA. A comparative risk assessment of burden of disease and injury attributable to 67 risk factors and risk factor clusters in 21 regions, 1990-2010: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. Lancet. 2012;380(9859):2224-60. MacArthur Foundation. Research network on SES and health [April 21, 2015]. Available from: http://www.macses.ucsf.edu/Research/Psychosocial/pss4.php.

(27)

26

Manenschijn L, Koper JW, Lamberts SW, van Rossum EF. Evaluation of a method to measure long term cortisol levels. Steroids. 2011;76(10-11):1032-6.

Manenschijn L, van Kruysbergen RG, de Jong FH, Koper JW, van Rossum EF. Shift work at young age is associated with elevated long-term cortisol levels and body mass index. The Journal of clinical endocrinology and metabolism. 2011;96(11):E1862-5.

Manenschijn L, Schaap L, van Schoor NM, van der Pas S, Peeters GM, Lips P, Koper JW, van Rossum EF. High long-term cortisol levels, measured in scalp hair, are associated with a history of

cardiovascular disease. The Journal of clinical endocrinology and metabolism. 2013;98(5):2078-83. Martikainen PT, Marmot MG. Socioeconomic differences in weight gain and determinants and consequences of coronary risk factors. The American journal of clinical nutrition. 1999;69(4):719-26. Matheson FI, Moineddin R, Glazier RH. The weight of place: a multilevel analysis of gender,

neighborhood material deprivation, and body mass index among Canadian adults. Social science & medicine. 2008;66(3):675-90.

Matthews KA, Gallo LC, Taylor SE. Are psychosocial factors mediators of socioeconomic status and health connections? A progress report and blueprint for the future. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2010;1186:146-73.

Merrill RM, Richardson JS. Validity of self-reported height, weight, and body mass index: findings from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, 2001-2006. Preventing chronic disease. 2009;6(4):A121.

Miller GE, Chen E, Zhou ES. If it goes up, must it come down? Chronic stress and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis in humans. Psychological bulletin. 2007;133(1):25-45.

Moore CJ, Cunningham SA. Social position, psychological stress, and obesity: a systematic review. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics. 2012;112(4):518-26.

Mouchacca J, Abbott GR, Ball K. Associations between psychological stress, eating, physical activity, sedentary behaviours and body weight among women: a longitudinal study. BMC public health. 2013;13:828.

Nguyen V, Peschard K. Anthropology, inequality, and disease: A review. Annual Review of Anthropology. 2003;32:447-74.

Parks EP, Kumanyika S, Moore RH, Stettler N, Wrotniak BH, Kazak A. Influence of stress in parents on child obesity and related behaviors. Pediatrics. 2012;130(5):e1096-104.

Pudrovska T, Logan ES, Richman A. Early-life social origins of later-life body weight: the role of socioeconomic status and health behaviors over the life course. Social science research. 2014;46:59-71.

Purslow LR, Young EH, Wareham NJ, Forouhi N, Brunner EJ, Luben RN, Welch AA, Khaw KT, Bingham SA, Sandhu MS. Socioeconomic position and risk of short-term weight gain: prospective study of 14,619 middle-aged men and women. BMC public health. 2008;8:112.

Rask E, Olsson T, Soderberg S, Andrew R, Livingstone DE, Johnson O, Walker BR. Tissue-specific dysregulation of cortisol metabolism in human obesity. The Journal of clinical endocrinology and metabolism. 2001;86(3):1418-21.

Raul JS, Cirimele V, Ludes B, Kintz P. Detection of physiological concentrations of cortisol and cortisone in human hair. Clinical biochemistry. 2004;37(12):1105-11.

Robert SA, Reither EN. A multilevel analysis of race, community disadvantage, and body mass index among adults in the US. Soc Sci Med. 2004;59(12):2421-34.

Ross C, Reynolds J, Geis K. The Contingent Meaning of Neighborhood Stability for Residents' Psychological Well-Being. American Sociological Review. 2000;65(4):581-97.

Russell E, Koren G, Rieder M, Van Uum S. Hair cortisol as a biological marker of chronic stress: current status, future directions and unanswered questions. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37(5):589-601.

Sauve B, Koren G, Walsh G, Tokmakejian S, Van Uum SH. Measurement of cortisol in human hair as a biomarker of systemic exposure. Clinical and investigative medicine Medecine clinique et

(28)

27

Schlotz W, Kumsta R, Layes I, Entringer S, Jones A, Wust S. Covariance between psychological and endocrine responses to pharmacological challenge and psychosocial stress: a question of timing. Psychosomatic medicine. 2008;70(7):787-96.

Schulz AJ, House JS, Israel BA, Mentz G, Dvonch JT, Miranda PY, Kannan S, Koch M. Relational pathways between socioeconomic position and cardiovascular risk in a multiethnic urban sample: complexities and their implications for improving health in economically disadvantaged populations. Journal of epidemiology and community health. 2008;62(7):638-46.

Shankardass K, McConnell R, Jerrett M, Lam C, Wolch J, Milam J, Gilliland F, Berhane K. Parental stress increases body mass index trajectory in pre-adolescents. Pediatric obesity. 2014;9(6):435-42. Stalder T, Steudte S, Alexander N, Miller R, Gao W, Dettenborn L, Kirschbaum C. Cortisol in hair, body mass index and stress-related measures. Biological psychology. 2012;90(3):218-23.

Stalder T, Kirschbaum C, Alexander N, Bornstein SR, Gao W, Miller R, Stark S, Bosch JA, Fischer JE. Cortisol in hair and the metabolic syndrome. The Journal of clinical endocrinology and metabolism. 2013;98(6):2573-80.

Steptoe A, Feldman PJ. Neighborhood problems as sources of chronic stress: development of a measure of neighborhood problems, and associations with socioeconomic status and health. Annals of behavioral medicine : a publication of the Society of Behavioral Medicine. 2001;23(3):177-85. Steptoe A, Wardle J, Pollard TM, Canaan L, Davies GJ. Stress, social support and health-related behavior: a study of smoking, alcohol consumption and physical exercise. Journal of psychosomatic research. 1996;41(2):171-80.

Thomson S, Koren G, Fraser LA, Rieder M, Friedman TC, Van Uum SH. Hair analysis provides a historical record of cortisol levels in Cushing's syndrome. Experimental and clinical endocrinology & diabetes : official journal, German Society of Endocrinology [and] German Diabetes Association. 2010;118(2):133-8.

Tryon MS, Stanhope KL, Epel ES, Mason AE, Brown R, Medici V, Havel PJ, Laugero KD. Excessive Sugar Consumption May Be a Difficult Habit to Break: A View From the Brain and Body. The Journal of clinical endocrinology and metabolism. 2015;100(6):2239-47.

van Lenthe FJ, Mackenbach JP. Neighbourhood deprivation and overweight: the GLOBE study. International journal of obesity and related metabolic disorders : journal of the International Association for the Study of Obesity. 2002;26(2):234-40.

Van Ryzin MJ, Chatham M, Kryzer E, Kertes DA, Gunnar MR. Identifying atypical cortisol patterns in young children: The benefits of group-based trajectory modeling. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34(1):50-61.

Van Uum SH, Sauve B, Fraser LA, Morley-Forster P, Paul TL, Koren G. Elevated content of cortisol in hair of patients with severe chronic pain: a novel biomarker for stress. Stress. 2008;11(6):483-8. Vanaelst B, Michels N, Clays E, Herrmann D, Huybrechts I, Sioen I, Vyncke K, De Henauw S. The association between childhood stress and body composition, and the role of stress-related lifestyle factors--cross-sectional findings from the baseline ChiBSD survey. International journal of behavioral medicine. 2014;21(2):292-301.

Veldhorst MA, Noppe G, Jongejan MH, Kok CB, Mekic S, Koper JW, van Rossum EF, van den Akker EL. Increased scalp hair cortisol concentrations in obese children. The Journal of clinical endocrinology and metabolism. 2014;99(1):285-90.

Wardle J, Chida Y, Gibson EL, Whitaker KL, Steptoe A. Stress and adiposity: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Obesity. 2011;19(4):771-8.

Wardle J, Gibson EL. Stress and the heart: Psychosocial pathways to coronary heart disease. London: BMJ Books; c2002. Impact of stress on diet: processes and implications; p. 124-49.

Wennig R. Potential problems with the interpretation of hair analysis results. Forensic science international. 2000;107(1-3):5-12.

Wester VL, Staufenbiel SM, Veldhorst MA, Visser JA, Manenschijn L, Koper JW, Klessens-Godfroy FJ, van den Akker EL, van Rossum EF. Long-term cortisol levels measured in scalp hair of obese patients. Obesity. 2014;22(9):1956-8.

(29)

28

Zellner DA, Loaiza S, Gonzalez Z, Pita J, Morales J, Pecora D, Wolf A. Food selection changes under stress. Physiology & behavior. 2006;87(4):789-93.

(30)

29

Table 1. Characteristics of women and children (n=70 women and n=30 maternal-child pairsa) All women (n=70) Maternal-child pairs (n=30)

Women’s characteristics

Age: mean (SD), range 43.4 (7.2), 23.7-51.1 45.9 (3.9), 37.9-51.1

Born in Australia: n (%) 65 (92.9) 28 (93.3) Marital status: n (%) Married/de facto 62 (88.6) 27 (90.0) Previously married 4 (5.7) 3 (10.0) Never married 4 (5.7) 0 (0.0) Education: n (%)

Did not complete high school 13 (18.6) 3 (10.0)

Completed high school 28 (40.0) 15 (50.0)

Tertiary 29 (41.4) 12 (40.0)

Household income: n (%)

< $1500 per week 29 (41.4) 11 (36.7)

≥ $1500 per week 32 (45.7) 15 (50.0)

Undisclosed 9 (12.9) 4 (13.3)

≥ 1 child living in the home: n (%) 56 (80.0) 30.0 (100.0) Employment status: n (%)

Full-time 30 (42.9) 9 (30.0)

Part-time 29 (41.4) 19 (63.3)

Not in paid employment 11 (15.7) 2 (6.7)

Smoking status: n (%) Never 45 (64.3) 16 (53.3) Former 21 (30.0) 12 (40.0) Current 4 (5.7) 2 (6.7) Weight status: n (%) Healthy weight 33 (47.1) 13 (43.3) Overweight 24 (34.3) 10 (33.3) Obese 13 (18.6) 7 (23.3)

BMI: mean (SD), range 26.2 (6.0), 18.9-46.7 27.4 (7.0), 18.9-46.7 Hair cortisol level (ng/g): mean (SD),

range

123.7 (71.2), 42.5-388.8

117.1 (51.3), 45.9-227.3 Number of cortisol measures taken: n

(%)

1 2 (2.9) 0 (0.0)

2 3 (4.3) 2 (6.7)

3 65 (92.9) 28 (93.3)

Hair has been coloured: n (%) 50 (71.4) 20 (66.7)

(31)

30

Children’s characteristics

n=30

Age: mean (SD), range 14.3 (3.9), 10.4-17.8

Sex: n (%)

Male 17 (56.7)

Female 13 (43.3)

zBMI: mean (SD), range 0.3 (1.1), -1.59-2.9

Weight status: n (%)

Healthy weight 21 (70.0)

Overweight 6 (20.0)

Obese 3 (10.0)

Hair cortisol level (ng/g): mean (SD), range

96.6 (49.6), 41.9-237.7 Number of cortisol measures taken: n

(%)

1 3 (10.0)

2 2 (6.7)

3 25 (83.3)

Hair has been coloured: n (%) 0 (0)

BMI: body mass index; PSS: perceived stress scale; zBMI: age and sex-adjusted body mass index

aA total of 70 women were included in the analysis. Of these women, 30 were also included

(32)

31

Table 2. Associations between hair cortisol levels, Perceived Stress Scale scores, and body mass index

Outcome Predictor Crude model Adjusted modela

β 95% CI p β 95% CI p

Women (n=70)

Women’s BMI Women’s cortisol 0.20 -2.59, 2.99 0.888 0.13 -2.34, 2.59 0.918 Women’s PSS score Women’s cortisol 0.30 -0.77, 1.37 0.579 0.14 -0.91, 1.18 0.795

Women’s BMI Women’s PSS score 0.71 0.14, 1.28 0.015 0.65 0.06, 1.23 0.032

Maternal-child pairs (n=30)

Children’s zBMI Maternal cortisol -0.07 -0.75, 0.61 0.832 -0.22 -0.91, 0.45 0.496 Children’s cortisol Maternal cortisol 0.49 0.15, 0.83 0.006 0.51 0.17, 0.84 0.004 Children’s zBMI Maternal PSS score 0.14 -0.06, 0.34 0.171 0.12 -0.06, 0.29 0.182 Children’s cortisol Maternal PSS score 0.05 -0.06, 0.07 0.878 -0.01 -0.11, 0.08 0.777 Children’s zBMI Children’s cortisol 0.15 -0.76, 1.06 0.739 0.20 -0.85, 1.25 0.694 BMI: body mass index; PSS: Perceived Stress Scale; zBMI: age and sex-adjusted body mass index.

Findings represent effect estimates from linear regression models using robust standard errors; statistically significant results shown in bold.

aModels for women controlled for education, smoking status and number of children living in the home, models for children controlled for

(33)

32 FIGURE CAPTIONS

Figure 1

A Scatterplot of women’s hair cortisol levels and their body mass index (n=70 participants).

In adjusted linear regression analyses, women’s hair cortisol levels were not associated with

their body mass index(β = 0.13;95% CI = -2.34, 2.59; p = 0.918).

B Scatterplot of women’s perceived stress scale scores and their body mass index (n=70 participants). In adjusted linear regression analyses, women’s perceived stress scale scores

were positively associated with their body mass index (β = 0.65; 95% CI = 0.06, 1.23; p =

0.032).

C Scatterplot of maternal hair cortisol levels and their child’s hair cortisol levels (n=30 maternal-child pairs). In adjusted linear regression analyses, mothers’ hair cortisol levels

were positively associated with their child’s hair cortisol levels(β = 0.51; 95% CI = 0.17,

References

Related documents

The objective of this study was to compare the BAI and body mass index (BMI) as measurements of body adiposity and metabolic risk.. Materials and Methods: This was a

The purpose of this study was to investigate associations between combinations of body mass index (BMI) cate- gories plus non-alcoholic fatty liver disease (NAFLD) and diabetes

The body mass index (BMI) has become the standard parameter used to predict body fat when all that is known is the subject's weight and height. However, the BMI has

Body mass index (BMI) may be a prognostic factor for gastric cancer with peritoneal dissemination RESEARCH Open Access Body mass index (BMI) may be a prognostic factor for gastric cancer

ACTH: Adrenocorticotropic hormone; AI: Alternative Intervention; BMI: Body Mass Index; CAPS-CA-5: Clinical Administered PTSD Scale for DSM-5 Child/Ado- lescent Version; CAR:

AKI: Acute kidney injury; APACHE: Acute Physiology and Chronic Health Evaluation; BMI: Body mass index; CCI: Charlson comorbidity index score; CI: Confidence interval; CKD:

Objectıve : To evaluate the effect of Body Mass Index (BMI) on arterial blood pressure response in patients undergoing stress exercise testing.. Systolic blood pressure (BP)

Some cases and fitness is based on this content does not work and risk because there was elected chief resident education and find your body mass index bmi table above