• No results found

Photosensitivity of the Frog Iris

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Photosensitivity of the Frog Iris"

Copied!
17
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

Photosensitivity of the Frog Iris

L L O Y D B A R R and M A T H E W A L P E R N

From the Departments of Physiology and Ophthalmology, The University of Michigan, Ann Arbor

A B S T R A C W T h e absorption of q u a n t a by rhodopsin leads to the contraction of frog iris muscle. T h e contractions reach a m a x i m u m after a b o u t 8 sec. in the light. W h e n the light is turned off the irises relax exponentially with a half-time of about 6 sec. M e m b r a n e polarization is not necessary for the response but calcium m o v e m e n t and m e m b r a n e permeability changes p r o b a b l y are. T h e response is not mediated by acetylcholine or epinephrine. T h e curves of log I t vs. log t for constant response amplitude bend progressively u p w a r d away from a unit slope line at short times as larger response criteria are used because (a) light influences tension development over longer times and (b) the higher in- tensity, shorter duration flashes are less effective.

P u p i l l a r y r e s p o n s e s t o l i g h t i n f r o g ( a n d i n o t h e r a m p h i b i a a s w e l l as t e l e o s t s , e s p e c i a l l y e e l s ) o c c u r e v e n w h e n t h e e y e s h a v e b e e n r e m o v e d f r o m t h e b o d y . A r n o l d is s a i d t o h a v e d e s c r i b e d t h i s e f f e c t i n 1841.1 B r o w n - S e q u a r d ( 1 8 4 7 a, 1 Arnold is cited by Steinach:

"Fr. Arnold, Physiologic II Bd. 1841" and by Magnus:

"F. Arnold, Physiologic Bd. 2, 1841"

without further page references. According to Steinach, these experiments were carried out on iso- lated eels' eyes. We have not been able to document the reference. The most likely place is:

Friedrich Arnold, Lehrbuch der Physiologic des Menschen, Zweiter Theil, Zweite Abtheilung, Zfirich Bei Orell, F/issli und Compagnie, 1841.

We have examined this book in some detail, but cannot find any reference to the experiments which Steinach describes. On the contrary, near the top of page 652 we find " . . . Da, wie diess mehrere Physiologen (Hailer, Zinn, Fontana, Caldrani, Lambert) durch Versuch ermittelt haben, das nnmittlebar auf die Iris durch die kleine Oeffnung eines Kartenblatts fallende, mehr oder weniger concentrirte Licht keine Ver~.nderung in der Pupille hervorbringt, sondern diese sich erst dann verengert, wenn die Lichtstrahlen durch das Sehloch auf den Hintergrund des Auges gelan- g e n ; . . . "

Moreover we have not been able to find any evidence that Arnold had changed his mind on this question even as late as 1851. In his

Handbuch der Anatomic des Menschen, Band 2, 2 Abtheilung, Freiburg im Breisgau, Herder'sche Verlagshandlung, 1851

we note on page 1029 (in small print) the following:

" U e b e r die Einflusse und Zfistande, welche eine verschiedene Ver~inderung der Pupille bewirken, vergl, meine Phys. Th. II s. 643 i T . . . 2'

I249

The Journal of General Physiology

on May 2, 2019

jgp.rupress.org

Downloaded from

http://doi.org/10.1085/jgp.46.6.1249

(2)

x 2 5 o T H E J O U R N A L O F G E N E R A L P H Y S I O L O G Y • V O L U M E 4 6 • I963

184:7 b,

1859) a iew years later showed that the phenomenon was due to light

which fell on the iris. Furthermore, he tound that the response persisted after lethal doses of strychnine, ether, opium, or atropine and that it was not due to an increase in temperature in the iris.

Steinach (1892) reviewed and extended these earlier observations. He pointed out that only the internal edge of the iris must be illuminated and that the contraction spreads from one side of the iris to the other. He found the greatest response using the blue-green part of the spectrum but considered from the color of the iris granules that the blue-violet fraction was really the most effective. Finally, he found that the response was increased by storing the frogs for days or weeks in a dark cellar.

Using somewhat more reliable techniques, Magnus (1899) also found that the largest responses were elicited by the green light of a prism spectrum and

suggested that Kiihne's (1879)

"Sehpurpur" (i.e.,

rhodopsin) was the absorbing

pigment. He found that atropine affected the response to photic stimulation more than the response to electric stimulation and concluded that the con- traction of the iris sphincter was not due to a direct effect on muscle fibers but was by way of nervous elements within the iris. Both Steinach and Magnus eliminated the possibility of retinal involvement by observing that pupillary constriction still occurred when the iris was dissected free.

The possibility that the results described by Magnus were a consequence of an irreversible damage to t h e muscle due to the high concentrations of atropine employed was suggested by Guth (1901) who showed that the re- sponses both to light and electricity diminish together.

The first modern attempt to define the action spectrum by Weale (1956) resulted in a sensitivity curve which decreased monotonically with wave length between 420 and 600 m#, a fact which directly contradicts the results

obtained in the last century

(i.e.,

that the wave length for maximum effect

coincided with the absorption peak of rhodopsin). More recently Seliger (1962) observed the change in diameter of isolated eel irises in response to long flashes. He corroborated Steinach's and Magnus' result that the maxi- m u m response is obtained at about 500 m#.

In view of the experiments just summarized, the characteristics of the re- sponse of the iris to light (action spectrum, time course, dependence on time in the dark, effect of variation of flash duration, and intensity) ought to be determined clearly and in detail.

The experiments discussed in this paper were designed to establish the photokinetics and a definitive action spectrum of the mechanical response. They also provide data which bear on the plausibility of events which might mediate the response, (a) changes of the state of the membrane, (b) release of cholinergic or adrenergic neurohumors, (¢) movements of calcium.

(3)

LLOYD BAVa~ -~D MAa'~w ALPZRN Iris Photoresporue x25I

M E T H O D S

Frog iris preparations free of corneal, scleral, ciliary muscle components were made by cutting around the periphery of the iris. Most of the attachments to the ciliary body were removed, but some were left to avoid damage to the delicate iris. The irises were mounted between platinum hooks passed through the pupils. Sometimes two irises were mounted in parallel between double hooks. The tension developed was recorded on a Grass polygraph via a 3 inch balsam lever attached to a Grass tension transducer. Shortening was less than 2 per cent of initial length (about 5 ram) and identical records were obtained with much longer or heavier levers. The mechanical system was peri- odically calibrated with standard weights in the appropriate range.

Frogs were kept in dark, cool chambers and had access to running water. They were kept up to 2 weeks and fed strained liver every other day.

The chambers in which the irises were bathed were of Pyrex and Herasll quartz. The monochromatic stimulating light was provided by a mercury vapor light source or by a tungsten ribbon filament lamp (color temperature of about 3000°C) which then passed through a 1200 lines/ram Bausch and Lomb grating monochromator, with a 500 mm focal length. The exit slit was imaged with unit magnification by a quartz lens mounted about 10 cm away from the monochromator. The iris prepara- tion was mounted in the plane of this image and was always smaller than the image of the slit. The energy of the monochromatic light at each wave band through the spec- trum was determined using a lamp calibrated at the National Bureau of Standards and a thermopile. The intensity of the light was varied by varying the height of the entrance slit. White light was provided by a tungsten filament of about the same color temperature imaged on the preparation. A calibrated iris diaphragm immediately in front of the imaging lens served to vary the intensity, in this case.

The normal Ringer's solution comprised 112 w_M NaC1, 1.87 mM KCI, 1.1 m_M CaC12, 2.38 m_M NaHCOa, 0.072 m_M NaH2PO~, and 11.1 mM glucose.

The K2SO4-substituted Ringer's solution was the same except that 74.7 rras K2SO4 was substituted for the 112 mM NaC1; in addition, KHCO8 and KH2PO4 replaced NaHCOa and NaH2PO4 mole for mole. The solutions containing different amounts of CaCI~, EDTA, or citrate were prepared to have the same osmolarity as the control solutions by varying the NaC1 or K2SO4 concentrations. All solutions were at pH 7.3 and the bath was at room temperatures (22-25°C).

R E S U L T S

Individual Responses

W h e n the isolated iris is exposed to light, it generates tension after a short latent period (Fig. 1 A). This is true even for exposures shorter t h a n the latent period. Increases in the durations of short flashes result in larger responses, b u t the light becomes progressively less effective as it is prolonged. I n fact, as flash d u r a t i o n is increased beyond about 8 see. the rate of tension development

(4)

I252

A

T H E J O U R N A L O F G E N E R A L P H Y S I O L O G Y • V O L U M E 46 • 1963 F L A S H I N T E N S I T Y 0.74 x I0 ~ L U M E N S M E T E R S "z 3 3 Sec. ~ 1 . 0

~

0 . 0 "

I ] I I I I 1 I I I I I I I I I I I I I 1

2 SECONDS

B

~

~ ~ L A S M

DURATION 2 SEC,

I = 0.23 x 103

I

I

1

I

I

1

I

I

I

I

]

I

I

1

I

I

I

I

SECOND

F I o u ~ I. (A) Isometric contractions of the frog iris resulting from flashes of white light of the same intensity but of different durations. The number on each curve repre- sents flash duration. The separation of the marks on the time scale is 2 sec. (B) Isometric contractions resulting from flashes of white light of the same duration but of different in- tensities. Same tension scale but different time scale. Note that the response to a 2 sec., 0.74 X l03 lumen m -~ flash appears in both (A) and (B).

d u r i n g t h e r e s p o n s e decreases u n t i l finally t h e iris a c t u a l l y b e g i n s to relax. T h u s , for v e r y l o n g flashes t h e t e n s i o n b e i n g d e v e l o p e d b y t h e iris passes t h r o u g h a m a x i m u m a n d falls off, a p p r o a c h i n g e x p o n e n t i a l l y a v a l u e a b o v e t h e r e s t i n g tension. W h e n a flash e n d s t h e m u s c l e c o n t i n u e s to d e v e l o p t e n s i o n (short flashes) or m a i n t a i n t e n s i o n (long flashes) for a b o u t a s e c o n d b e f o r e r e l a x i n g .

I n r e s p o n s e to flashes of t h e s a m e i n t e n s i t y b u t d i f f e r e n t d u r a t i o n s , t e n s i o n d e v e l o p m e n t a l w a y s follows t h e s a m e initial t i m e course e v e n t h o u g h t h e

(5)

LLOYD BARR AND MATHEW ALPERN ~isPhotore~o~e z253

shortest flashes are over before the muscle contracts and the longest persist long after the m a x i m u m tension has been achieved (Fig. 1 A). I n the comple- mentary experiment, holding duration fixed and varying the intensity, the initial rate of tension development and the peak tension increases approxi-

/ <t (J ~o (.9 o / z o ¢/) z LIJ I-- 1.0 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 ! 0.1 0 . 0 5 0 . 0 4 0 . 0 3 0 . 0 2 m •°.° o• O • O O* • " " ' . . O 6 O • • . _ _ * o ( P ( ~ ) O o - o o o o o 0 - ~ o o o b O A o 7.28 ( 1 0 ~ L u x • 41.2 (10)SLux .0.74 (I0) 5 L u x o - - o o 00.23 (10) 5 Lux R O roof Lfl 0 ¢ 75t-- LI.I n 501-- Z O • I = 0.23 (I O} 5 LUX B o I = 0.74 (10) 5 Lux • R ~ x 1 =7.28 (10) 5 Lux

"~X~.

v I =41.2 (10)" Lux " v 9 , $ F I 0 s h D u r o t i 0 n 2 Sec. x v , ~ . o(. ,P ,,

I ~

251- I- o l ~ J ~ I 2 s e c . I 0 . 0 1 T I M E " - - ' - ~

FIOURE 9. (A) T h e responses s h o w n in Fig. 1B plotted w i t h the l o g a r i t h m of the tension d e v e l o p e d as the o r d i n a t e . B o t h the initial rise a n d fall of tension follow simple e x p o n e n - tial t i m e courses w i t h t i m e constants i n d e p e n d e n t of intensity. (B) T h e responses s h o w n i n Fig. 1B plotted w i t h p e r c e n t of m a x i m u m tension d e v e l o p e d d u r i n g the t w i t c h as t h e o r d i n a t e . T h e t i m e course of t h e responses is i n d e p e n d e n t of intensity.

mately linearly with the logarithm of the light intensity (Figs. 1 B and 4 A). If the tension time course curves in Fig. 1 B are plotted using a logarithmic tension scale (Fig. 2 A), b o t h t h e initial rise of tension and the relaxation decay are seen as simple exponential functions of time. For these responses the time it takes to achieve any given fraction of the peak tension of the twitch is independent of intensity. T h e responses to flashes of different intensities, normalized for the peak tension, superimpose (Fig. 2 B).

(6)

z254 T H E J O U R N A L O F G E N E R A L P H Y S I O L O G Y * V O L U M E 4 6 • ~963

Fig. 3 illustrates the m a x i m u m tension developed during a twitch as a function of flash duration and intensity. For any given intensity the peak tension increases monotonically with duration. F r o m Fig. 1 A it is evident that the curve in Fig. 3 for any intensity will become a horizontal straight line as soon as the duration of the flash is longer t h a n the m i n i m u m duration required for m a x i m u m peak tension (Tm~=) for that intensity; since at a n y given du- ration, the tension time course is independent of intensity (Fig. 2 B) the

I00-[ ~, ~ 41.2 (I07 Lumens meter-2z,

~ •

i

,,

.

a:: • 2.52 o ~ 6 0 8 o ~, o 0.74 u x x

i

0 g ab Jg ~o 15 ~o~c.

DURATION OF THE FLASHES

FIGURE 3. The maximum tensions of twitches of two preparations, as per cent of maxi- m u m tension developed by the muscle, plotted as a function of the durations of the flashes for several intensities. For each intensity there is a maximum attainable tension reacheP after about 8 sec.

m i n i m u m flash duration for Tm,x is always the same. T h e smooth curve for each intensity in Fig. 3 is d r a w n to indicate these trends of the data.

T h e d a t a in Fig. 3 m a y be used to find a relation, for a n y given duration, between peak tension during a twitch (T) and intensity. To do this it is best to interpolate between the points to find the peak tensions at a constant du- ration. This can be facilitated by finding that transformation of coordinates for which the results most nearly fall on a straight line. This is a plot of

logarithm (1 -- T~ Tm,x) vs. flash duration. Fig. 4 A shows the relation between

T a n d log I for these interpolated flash durations. Linear extrapolation of these d a t a to zero tension shows that the threshold intensity, even for 0.1 sec. flashes, is independent of duration. This means that the critical duration for threshold flashes is less than 0.1 sec.

Fig. 4 B demonstrates in the traditional way (Hartline, 1934) the a m o u n t of light required to produce a given criterion response as a function of flash du- ration. For each of the criteria illustrated in this figure, the peak tension is not influenced by prolonging the longer flashes. Data points for which this is true

(7)

LLOYD BAmt AND MAxI-mW ALPzm~ Iris Photoresponse ~255

plot on a straight line of unit positive slope (I = constant). Since all the flash durations used exceed the critical d u r a t i o n for a threshold response, t h e threshold criterion d a t a all faU on such a time-independent unit slope line. As the response criterion is increased, however, light continues to be capable of influencing tension development, even t h o u g h it has been shining as long as 8 sec. F u r t h e r m o r e , because light is less effective at higher intensities t h a n at lower ones ( T ~ k In/, see Fig. 4 A), m o r e light is required in shorter flashes t h a n in longer ones to evoke big contractions. For these reasons the curves in Fig. 4 B based on progressively larger response criteria bend progressively up- w a r d a w a y from a unit slope line as the flash duration is reduced. T h e a m o u n t of light required to give a constant response in the neighborhood of three- quarters of a milligram is constant for flash durations up to 2 sec.

EJlect of Prior Illumination

W h a t is the time course of the recovery of iris photosensitivity following exposure to a strong light stimulus? T o answer this, dark-adapted irises were bleached by an intense light (4.12 X 104 lumen m -~) for about 20 rains. T h e irises contracted to a tension m a x i m u m in about 8 sec. and thereafter the tension exponentially d e c a y e d to about one-third its peak during the 20 min. in the light. W h e n the bleach light was t u r n e d off, the tension was m a i n t a i n e d for about a second and then decayed exponentially to the base level.

T h e recovery of photosensitivity was subsequently determined by obtaining responses to 10 sec. test flashes in the dark. I n any given recovery experiment an iris was tested only three or four times, but it was possible to obtain re- sponses at nine different intervals in the dark by repetition of the process with the test flashes interspersed at different times.

T h e means (4- SEM) of the ratios of test flash peak tensions to peak tension during the bleach, as a function of time in the dark, obtained from five irises are plotted in Fig. 5. It is evident that after about 2 rain. in the dark the tension developed is 60 per cent of the m a x i m u m tension developed during the bleach. T h e time course of recovery of photoresponsivity was not in- fluenced by using different test flashes (weak m o n o c h r o m a t i c or white light of different intensities and duration), suggesting that the procedure obviated possible distortions of the recovery curve due to the test flash.

T h e recovery of muscle photoresponsivity is similar to the recovery of the electroretinogram of the frog following strong bleaches. I n Fig. 5 the lines show measurements of the latter process by Wald, Brown, and K e n n e d y (1957).

Latent Period

T h e latent periods of the responses to light were measured u n d e r all experi- mental conditions. T h e grand m e a n for control dark-adapted irises was 0.77

(8)

,~5 6 I-- 4

t

4.0 I n.- LU I-- la3 Eb Z o 3.0 ILl 03 7 LU --I Z - 2.0 H 0 _ J 1.0 T H E J O U R N A L O F G E N E R A L P H Y S I O L O G Y • V O L U M E 46 • z963 • 0.1 sec. v 0 2 x

_: ,Oo"

. ~ /

: 240

j .

:

,OoOo

j , " ~ J "

.o~

2 3 4. LOG I ( I IN L U M E N S M E T E R - 2 )

B

\

o.75~s ~ . ~ S / o

t

- 9 , / o / / / # i / / O/Threshold I I [ - 1.0 0.0 1.0 LOG F L A S H D U R A T I O N t (t IN SECONDS) FIc. 4-

(9)

L L O Y D BARR AND 1V~ATHEW ALPERN Iris Photoresponse I257 ± 0.31 ( S D ) sec. w i t h a r a n g e o f 0.43 t o 2.4 sec. T h e r e w a s n o s t a t i s t i c a l l y s i g n i f i c a n t c o r r e l a t i o n b e t w e e n l a t e n t p e r i o d a n d l i g h t i n t e n s i t y o r a m p l i t u d e o f t h e r e s p o n s e o v e r t h e r a n g e o f b r i e f flashes used. T h e r e w e r e n o s i g n i f i c a n t c h a n g e s o f l a t e n t p e r i o d c a u s e d b y h i g h e x t e r n a l p o t a s s i u m , s c o p o l a m i n e , a t r o p i n e , p h y s o s t i g m i n e , c a r b a c h o l , o r a c e t y l c h o l i n e . H o w e v e r , as t h e c o n t r a c t i o n s w e r e a t t e n u a t e d t o a b o u t 95 p e r c e n t n o r m a l i n C a - f r e e c i t r a t e solutions, t h e l a t e n t p e r i o d i n c r e a s e d f r o m 1.02 ± 0 . 0 5 ( S E M ) t o 1.92 ± 0 . 0 6 ( S E M ) (t test p < 0.01). I 0 0 - 8 0 - I - z Lu o O~ , , , 6 0 - n >- I - ra > ~ 4 0 - 0 o. w 2 0 - 0 I[11] 0.1 s ~ n /

r - - / /

-I-

FROG IRIS M -+ S.E.M. Frog E.R.G. (Wold, Brown, and Kennedy, 1957)

- ~ Following 2 ( l O ) S m l for 15sec. Following 2(10) s ml for 50sec. i r l l v 1 [ i f I I J i l l [ T I

1.0 I 0

M I N U T E S IN T H E DARK

FmuRn 5. Recovery of sensitivity of five irises to light as a function of the time in the dark following exposure tO 41.2 (10) 8 lumen m ~ for 20 rain. The ordinate is the ratio of

peak twitch tension in a response to the test flash to the peak tension developed during the light adaptation exposure. The test flash in these experiments was of the same in- tensity as the adapting exposures. The solid and dotted lines show the recovery of sensi- tivity of the frog retina following adaptation exposure of the indicated luminance (in millilamberts) and duration as measured by Wald, Brown, and Kennedy (1957) with the electroretinogram.

A f t e r 20 rain. b l e a c h e s , as six irises r e c o v e r e d i n t h e d a r k , t h e a v e r a g e l a t e n t p e r i o d d e c r e a s e d p r o g r e s s i v e l y f r o m 1.36 ± 0 . 4 2 ( S E M ) a t 10 sec. t o 0 . 7 4 ±

FIOURE 4. (A) The maximum tensions of twitches plotted as a function of the logarithm of the intensity of the flashes for different durations. The data are computed from Fig. 3 with the maximum tension of each preparation arbitrarily equated to 5.2 mg (i.e., t h e maximum tension developed by either). That the curves for different flash durations all intersect the abscissa at nearly the same place indicates that there is an in- tensity threshold for the durations used. (B) The logarithm of the amount of light re- quired to elicit a given amount of tension plotted as a function of flash durations. Note that the critical duration for threshold tension is less than 0.1 sec. but that there is a critical duration (It = constant) of about 2 sec. for 0.75 mg responses.

(10)

I258 T H E J O U R N A L O F G ~ . N ~ . R A L P H Y S I O L O G Y • V O L U M E 46 • x963 0,06 (SEM) at 9 rain. T h e decrease was statistically significant. It had a Pearson p r o d u c t - m o m e n t coefficient r = - 0 . 3 1 (N = 77; p < 0.01). H o w - ever, there was no statistically significant change of latent period after 6 rain. in the dark, even though the tension m a x i m a of responses were still increasing at this time (see Fig. 5).

Action Spectrum

An action spectrum is properly c o m p a r e d with an absorption spectrum only when the action spectrum represents the reciprocal of the n u m b e r of q u a n t a

FIGURE 6. The action spectrum as measured by the reciprocal of the number of quanta required to produce a criterion tension level in three iris preparations. The points are the geometric means after the curves were vertically shifted for super- imposition with minimum scatter. The smooth curve is the extinction spectrum of frog rhodopsin as measured in vitro by Wald (1949). The sensitiv- ity measurements at 675 and 700 m/~ are probably erroneously high because of stray light in the monochromator. z 2 ' _o , Z I-- X Ul 0 I-- Z h i - J W I'- UJ ¢D

o,

0 0 0 0 0 o l l t b l l - l Frog Iris o 'rog Rhodopsin t W a l d , 1949) I 1 t o t f I I I

~?

:500 4 0 0 5 0 0 6 0 0 7 0 0 WAVE LENGTH m/=

at each wave length required to produce some constant physiological response under conditions where the magnitude of the response is proportional to the n u m b e r of incident quanta. T h e action spectrum of the frog iris determined in this w a y is illustrated in Fig. 6. T h e points are the means of measurements m a d e on three preparations and the line is the in vitro absorption spectrum of frog rhodopsin measured b y W a l d (1949).

T h e agreement between the two spectra in Fig. 6 is not very good. However, it is close enough to leave little d o u b t that the excitation of the iris muscle b y white fight is due to the absorption of q u a n t a b y rhodopsin.

Dependence on Calcium

Calcium seems to be involved twice in the sequence of processes which is initiated b y a stimulus to muscle and ends with contraction. First, excitable

(11)

LLOYD BARR AND MATHEW ALP~.RN Iris Photoresponse 1259 m e m b r a n e s are stabilized by calcium (Shanes, 1958). T h u s action potentials are elicited at lower stimulus strengths and indeed can be m a d e to appear spontaneously in skeletal muscle, when external calcium is decreased (Shanes, 1958; Bfilbring, H o l m a n , and Lullman, 1956). In addition, however, the con- traction itself seems to be d e p e n d e n t on an increase in intracellular calcium ions (Niedergerke, 1957; Bianchi, 1961). T h a t is, the coupling between the m e m b r a n e event a n d contraction involves calcium movement.

W h e n external calcium was slowly washed a w a y the strength of contraction of the iris in response to light increased before eventually declining. This result was obtained using simple calcium-free solutions or those which were calcium-

3.5 3.0 2.5 zE2.0 0 1.5 h i I-,- 1.0 0.5 X X ( X :~ing. X X X X X I Ringer's Na Citrate X X * X I Ringer's I 4 t O I 8 / I , I . I , , ~. t I I I Min. 0 120 160 200 240 TIME

FIGURE 7. The maximum tension of twitches elicited by standard 10 sec. flashes of white light [I = 41.2 (10) ~ lumen m -z] plotted against time when the incubating solution is either nor- mal Ringer's or calcium-free, citratec[ Ringer's. The twitch amplitude de- creases when calcium is washed out. Moreover, the decrease is much faster if the frequency of stimulation is. greater.

iree and contained in addition 2 m u E D T A or 2.67 mM citrate. T h e decline of contractility was too slow (time to half-amplitude when the iris was u n - stimulated was about an hour a n d a half) to be considered simply a function of sweeping calcium out of the extracellular space.

T h e possibility that calcium b o u n d in some m a n n e r was released by t h e light stimulus a n d then was effective in causing contraction was investigated in the following way. If there is a bound store of calcium and external calcium is washed away, then the rate of loss of contraction strength should be de- termined by the rate of stimulation.

Fig. 7 shows the amplitudes of the contractions w h e n an iris was (a) stimu- lated at a fast rate in a calcium-free bath, then (b) allowed to recover in a n o r m a l solution, then (¢) stimulated at a slow rate in a calcium-free bath. T h e figure shows clearly that it was not simply the time of washing out of the calcium alone which decreased the strength of the contraction but that the frequency of stimuli (contractions) was a strong d e t e r m i n a n t of the rate of loss of contractile strength. W h e n the order of t r e a t m e n t was reversed ((a) slow stimulus rate in Ca-free solutions; (b) recovery; (c) fast stimulus rate in

(12)

i26o THE JOURNAL OF GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY • VOLUME 46 • I963

Ca-free solutions), the result was exactly the same; i.e., the rate of loss of

strength of contraction again increased with the frequency of contractions in the calcium-free solution.

T h e rates of decrease of contractile force in calcium-free E D T A solutions were similar when either carbachol (10 -5 and 10 -s w / v ) or acetylcholine

(10 -6 and 10 -9 w / v ) was used as the stimulus.

ligl~ht 2 . 7 6 mg

l i g h t l i g h t . _ ^ / ~

p--I i.44mg ~f~2 ~Umg / \

C a r b ! l O - 4 Wash

I O " t i m e m o r k s , ~ , , , II , , , , , , ~ , s , , , , II , II , , , , i ,

16' gap 5' I0'

ligf~ht 0 . 7 9 mg / \ 8 mg ~,B.

l i g h t

Ringer's KaSO 4 Ringer's Carb. 10 " 4 K2SO 4 R. w a s h

, , , , , , II , , . . . . If , , , II , , , , ~ , ~ , , , , , ,11 , II , , ,

t

t

t

I

l

I I ' g a p 3 0 ' 10' ~' 40'

FIOURE 8. Record of isometric tension of isolated frog iris. Upper tracing shows the re- sponse to 10 sec. flashes before, during, and after exposure of the iris to 10 -4 w/v carbachol in normal Ringer's. Lower tracing shows the contraction caused by switching from a nor- mal Ringer bath to a K~SO4-substituted Ringer bath and also the iris response to light and carbachol in the presence of the K2SO4-Ringer's solution. The amplifier gain was turned down to record all of the large initial contraction to the K~SOrRinger's. On the time scales, the single prime marks refer to minutes and the double prime marks refer to seconds.

High External Potassium

Is a change in m e m b r a n e potential involved in photoexcitation? Is a change necessary? T h e results of the following experiments suggest that the answers are yes to the first question, but no to the second. W h e n an isolated iris is exposed to depolarizing KCL-Ringer's or K2SO4-Ringer's it contracts strongly. This contraction is not maintained indefinitely in the presence of high external potassium, but decays a w a y (see lower trace of Fig. 8). T h e iris still responds to light and drug stimuli in high potassium solution even t h o u g h it has com- pletely relaxed from the initial potassium contraction.

(13)

LLOYD BARR AND MATI-mW ALPERN Iris Photoresponse i26i solutions, the fact that the muscle still responds after as long as a h u n d r e d minutes in K~SO4-Ringer's indicates that an effective stimulus need not cause an appreciable change of m e m b r a n e potential. This in fact has been previously established for guinea pig intestinal muscle by Evans, Schild, and Thesleff (1958) using drug stimulation in high external potassium solutions and measuring the t r a n s m e m b r a n e potential. Since depolarizing a m e m b r a n e de- creases the response to light, however, there still seems to be a m e m b r a n e permeability change in the stimulus-response sequence. O n e might infer, therefore, that in a high sodium solution, generator potentials a n d action potentials are results of photic excitation. Preliminary experiments have shown that the rate of loss of radioactive potassium from irises is increased as a result of photic excitation in both high and lower external potassium solutions

(Barr, unpublished data).

Action of Drugs

T h e iris is said to comprise two histologically distinct smooth muscles, the radial dilator muscle and the circular constrictor muscle which forms a sphincter at the internal edge of the iris. T h e response to light reported here appeared from the mechanics of recording to be due to the contraction of the circular muscle. Certainly this is true, when, as in some preparations, the dilator muscle was dissected almost completely away. T h e response of the iris preparations to autonomic drugs verifies this.

T h e addition of 1-epinephrine (up to 10 -8 w / v ) or the less potent phenyl- ephrine (up to 10 -5 w / v ) to the muscle b a t h caused a very small decrease in tension. T h e presence of the highest concentrations of these drugs did not affect the response of the iris preparations to light. T h e addition of acetyl- choline to the muscle b a t h caused the iris to contract. Contractions due to concentrations of 10 -10 w / v acetylcholine were easily detected. T h e m a x i m u m response to acetylcholine was obtained at concentrations about 10 -7 w/v. However, the contractions due to the larger concentrations were quite vari- able. This is probably due to variability in the onset of the blocking action of the acetylcholine. T h e sensitivity of the preparations to carbachol was an o r d e r of m a g n i t u d e less t h a n to acetylcholine.

However, when the iris was contracting maximally in response to 10 -5 w / v acetylcholine or 10 -4 carbachol a flash of light still elicited a further con- traction (Fig. 8). T h e response to light still occurred after initial contraction to 10 -6 w / v acetylcholine was over. T h e tension~ development caused by fight in the presence of concentrations of acetylcholine where additional acetylcholine is ineffective is evidence against the possibility of mediation of the light re- sponse by acetylcholine. Furthermore, the iris developed a tachyphylaxis to acetylcholine and carbachol, but not to light.

(14)

1262 T H E J O U R N A L O F G E N E R A L P H Y S I O L O G Y • V O L U M E 46 • I963

sponse was obtained using other drugs (Table I). T h e light response was not potentiated by 10 -7 w / v physostigmine whereas the response to 10 -7 w / v acetylcholine was increased about 25 per cent. Concentrations of scopolamine (10 -7 w / v ) which completely blocked the iris response to 10 -e w / v acetyl- choline had no effect on the response to light. Although 10 -7 w / v atropine produced a 90 per cent block of a 10 -6 w / v acetylcholine response, it did not affect the light response. Procaine (10 -s w / v ) also did not affect the light re- sponse at the same time that it decreased the responses to acetylcholine to almost half control values.

T A B L E I

E F F E C T O F D R U G S O N I R I S R E S P O N S E

Conditioning d r u g Test stimulus Control tension*

~08r ¢,G/t t 10 -6 w / v e p i n e p h r i n e Light I00 10 -5 w / v p h e n y l e p h r i n e Light 100 10 -7 w / v physostigmine 10 -7 acetylcholine 125 10 -7 w / v physostigmine Light 100 10 -7 w / v scopolamine 10 -e acetylcholine 0 10 -~ w / v scopolamine L i g h t 100 10 -7 w / v a t r o p i n e 10 -6 aeetylcholine 10 10 -7 w / v a t r o p i n e Light 100 10 -5 w / v procaine 10 -e acetylcholine 60 10 -5 w / v procaine Light 100

* T h e figures in this column were c o m p u t e d as the ratio (in per cent) of the amplitude of the re- sponse to the test stimulus in the presence of the conditioning drug to t h e amplitude of the re- sponse to the test stimulus alone.

T h e response to light is potentiated by prior stimulation with cholinergic drugs and very low continuing stimulation with them. This is true even w h e n the m e m b r a n e is depolarized. Thus while it appears certain that the photo- response is not m e d i a t e d by acetylcholine, it is probable that cholinergic drugs a n d light share a c o m m o n locus of action, the m e m b r a n e .

D I S C U S S I O N

T h e deviation of the action spectrum points from the rhodopsin absorption spectrum in Fig. 6 is too great to be accounted for "simply by experimental error. It is, of course, conceivable that this difference between the iris action

spectrum and the in vitro absorption spectrum is due to slight differences in the

apoproteins or to some component of the intracellular environment.

A n o t h e r explanation is that a second pigment contributes to the iris action spectrum. This could occur in at least two ways. First, a pigment whose excitation does not lead to contraction is screening rhodopsin. Second, acti- vation of a pigment besides rhodopsin is contributing to the iris contraction.

(15)

LLOYD BARR AND MATI-mW AIa,~.RN Iris Photoresponse t263

I n this regard it should be kept in m i n d that some retinal rods of the frog do not contain rhodopsin. Since the discovery of rhodopsin (Boll, 1876) it has been realized that some retinal rods contain a different pigment which ab- sorbs strongly in blue light, is transparent in the green, but absorbs again in the yellow and red parts of the spectrum. Denton and Wyllie (1955) showed that the pigment (or pigments) in these "grass green" rods is photolabile in the blue but photostable in the yellow and red regions of the spectrum. Donner and Rushton (1959) proved that the absorption of light in the blue region of the spectrum by this pigment contributed to the physiological re- sponse eventually detectable in the discharge of retinal ganglion cells.

T o o little is known about the absorption spectrum of this green pigment(s) to make any predictions which are testable by the extent to which the iris action spectrum deviates from the rhodopsin absorption spectrum. However, the fact that the agreement in Fig. 6 is m u c h poorer in the blue than in the red region of the spectrum seems to support the idea that this additional pigment acts in the same way that rhodopsin does in the initiation of the iris photo- response.

The question of the chemical pathways between the photoactivated pigment and the excitation of the contractile apparatus must be considered entirely open. However, it seems certain that a membrane event and a movement of calcium ions occur.

T h e importance of calcium as the coupling agent is emphasized both by the dependence of tension on its presence and by the increase of the latent period when calcium is taken out of the system. Since the latent period is not affected by intensity or flash duration (> 0.1 sec.), it is concluded that the major portion of the time lag between the absorption of quanta and the onset of contraction is due to calcium-mediated "coupling" processes.

T h e results of the drug experiments rule out the possibility of adrenergic or cholinergic transmitters mediating the photoresponse. Nonetheless, the only pigment granules in the iris muscle cells look exactly like the melanin granules in the pigment epithelial cells (personal communication from M. M. Dewey). Thus the possibility remains that the rhodopsin resides in non-muscle cells which activate the muscle cells by way of irin, 5-hydroxytryptamine, or some other chemical transmitter. There are, however, certain difficulties with this hypothesis. If the response were mediated by some transmitter not so far investigated it might be expected that the time course of the responses evoked by light would be appreciably slowed in the presence of depolarizing po- tassium solutions. I n fact, it is found that although the peak twitch tension is reduced by a factor of about 2.5 in such solutions the latent periods and time courses of development and decay of tension remain unaffected by the de- polarization of the membrane.

(16)

I264 T H E J O U R N A L O F G E N E R A L P H Y S I O L O G Y • V O L U M E 46 • 1963

the a m o u n t of e n e r g y r e q u i r e d to evoke a response. O n e of the most sensitive iris p r e p a r a t i o n s g e n e r a t e d a b o u t 4 m g tension w h e n it was exposed to 12.5 ergs of blue-green light (X = 500 m/z). This is a considerably larger a m o u n t of e n e r g y t h a n a n y reasonable estimate of m e c h a n i c a l w o r k done. T h u s the question of w h e t h e r or n o t light e n e r g y is c o n v e r t e d into m e c h a n i c a l w o r k m u s t r e m a i n open.

We are indebted to Mrs. Ann Graves for her help with many of these experiments.

This work was assisted in part by United States Public Health Service Grant A-3819 and National Science Foundation Grant 10045.

Some of these experiments were described at the First International Biophysics Conference at Stock- holm, August 3, 1961.

Received for publication, Decemba 31, 1962.

B I B L I O G R A P H Y

BIANCHI, C. P., 1961, Calcium movements in striated muscle during contraction and contracture, in Biophysics of Physiological and Pharmacological Actions, (A. M. Shanes, editor), Washington, D. C., American Association for the Advancement of Science, 281.

BOLL, F., 1876, Sull'anatomia e fisiologia della retina, Mem. Accad. Lincei. Roma, 1,

371.

BROWN-SEQUARD, E., 1847 a, Recherches experimentales sur l'action de la lumiere et sur celle d ' u n changement de temperature sur l'iris, d a m les cinq classes d'animaux vertebres, Compt. rend. acad. So., 25,482.

BROWN-SEQUARD, E., 1847 b', Note complimentaire d ' u n memoire sur l'action de la lumiere et d ' u n changement de temperature sur 1'iris, Compt. rend. acad. Sc., 25,508. BROWN-SEQUARD, E., 1859, Recherches experimentales sur l'influence excitatrice de la lumiere, du froid et de la chaleur sur l'iris, dans les cinq classes d'animaux verte- bres, J. physiol., 2,281.

Bi~ILBRINC, E., HOLMAN, M., AND LULLMAN, H., 1956, Effects of calcium deficiency on striated muscle of the frog, or. Physiol., 133,101.

DENTON, E. J., AND WYLLm, J. H., 1955, Study of the photosensitive pigments in the pink and green rods of the frog, J. Physiol., 127, 81.

DONNER, K. O., AND RUSHTON, W. A. H., 1959, Rod-cone interaction in the frog's retina analysed by the Stiles-Crawford effect and by dark adaptation, J. Physiol.,

149,303.

EVANS, D. H. L., " S c H I L D , H. O., AND THESLEFF, S., 1958, Effects of drugs on de-

polarized plain muscle, .7. Physiol., 143,474.

GUTH, E., 1901, Untersuchungen fiber die directe motorische Wirkung des Lichtes auf den Sphincter pupillae des Aal- und Froschauges, Arch. ges. Physiol., 85, 119. HARTLINE, H. K., 1934, Intensity and duration in the excitation of single photo-

receptor units, or. Cell. and Cornp. Physiol. 5,229.

KOHNE, W., (1879), Chemische Vorgfinze in der Netzhaut. III. in Handbuch des Physiologic, (L. Hermann, editor), Leipzig, F. C. W. Vogel Verlag, 3, pt. 1,235.

(17)

LLOYD BARR AND MATHEW ALPERN MS Photoresponse 1265

MAGNUS, R.,

1899, Beitr/ige zur Ptipillarreaction des Aal- und Froschauges, Z. Biol.,

38,567.

NmDERGERKE, R., 1957, The rate of action of calcium ions on the contraction of the

heart, J. Physiol., 138,506.

SELmER, H. H., 1962, Direct action of light in naturally pigmented muscle fibers.

I. Action spectrum for contraction in eel iris sphincter, J. Gen. Physiol., 46,333.

SHANm, A. M., 1958, Electrochemical aspects of physiological and pharmacological

action in excitable cells. II. The action potential and excitation, Pharmacol. Rev.,

10, 165.

STEmACH, E., 1892, Untersuchungen zur vergleiehenden Physiologic der Iris. II,

Arch. ges. Physiol., 52,495.

WALl), G., 1949, The photochemistry of vision, Doe. Ophthalmol., 3, 94.

WALD, G., BROWN, P. K., AND I~NNEDY, D., 1957, Visual system of the alligator,

J. Gen. Physiol., 40,703.

W~AL~-, R. A., 1956, Observations on the direct effect of light on the irides of Rana

References

Related documents

Field experiments were conducted at Ebonyi State University Research Farm during 2009 and 2010 farming seasons to evaluate the effect of intercropping maize with

The paper is discussed for various techniques for sensor localization and various interpolation methods for variety of prediction methods used by various applications

This essay asserts that to effectively degrade and ultimately destroy the Islamic State of Iraq and Syria (ISIS), and to topple the Bashar al-Assad’s regime, the international

Eksperimenti su pokazali da je za mutiranu tRNA Trp , koja nosi supstituciju u D-ruci, smanjena točnost procesa translacije na način da su reakcije koje se odvijaju

Мөн БЗДүүргийн нохойн уушгины жижиг гуурсанцрын хучуур эсийн болон гөлгөр булчингийн ширхгийн гиперплази (4-р зураг), Чингэлтэй дүүргийн нохойн уушгинд том

19% serve a county. Fourteen per cent of the centers provide service for adjoining states in addition to the states in which they are located; usually these adjoining states have

Results suggest that the probability of under-educated employment is higher among low skilled recent migrants and that the over-education risk is higher among high skilled

This conclusion is further supported by the following observations: (i) constitutive expression of stdE and stdF in a Dam + background represses SPI-1 expression (Figure 5); (ii)