• No results found

Internet-delivered cognitive control training as a preventive intervention for remitted depressed patients : protocol for a randomized controlled trial

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Internet-delivered cognitive control training as a preventive intervention for remitted depressed patients : protocol for a randomized controlled trial"

Copied!
29
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

1 2

Internet-delivered cognitive control training as a preventive intervention for remitted 3

depressed patients: Protocol for a randomized controlled trial 4

5 6

Kristof Hoorelbeke*, Lien Faelens, Jeffrey Behiels, and Ernst H.W. Koster 7

Ghent University, Belgium 8 9 10 11 *Corresponding author: 12 Kristof Hoorelbeke 13

Department of Experimental Clinical and Health Psychology 14 Ghent University 15 Henri Dunantlaan 2 16 B-9000 Gent, Belgium 17 +32 9-264-94-16 18 e-mail: Kristof.Hoorelbeke@UGent.be 19 20 Lien Faelens 21

Department of Experimental Clinical and Health Psychology 22 Ghent University 23 Henri Dunantlaan 2 24 B-9000 Gent, Belgium 25 e-mail: Lien.Faelens@UGent.be 26 27

(2)

Jeffrey Behiels 28

Department of Experimental Clinical and Health Psychology 29 Ghent University 30 Henri Dunantlaan 2 31 B-9000 Gent, Belgium 32 e-mail: Jeffrey.Behiels@UGent.be 33 34 Ernst Koster 35

Department of Experimental Clinical and Health Psychology 36 Ghent University 37 Henri Dunantlaan 2 38 B-9000 Gent, Belgium 39 + 32 9 264 64 46 40 e-mail: Ernst.Koster@UGent.be 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55

(3)

Abstract 56

Background: Preventing recurrence of depression forms an important challenge for current 57

treatments. Cognitive control impairments often remain present during remission of 58

depression, putting remitted depressed patients at heightened risk for new depressive episodes 59

by disrupting emotion regulation processes. Importantly, research indicates that cognitive 60

control training targeting working memory functioning shows potential in reducing 61

maladaptive emotion regulation and depressive symptomatology in clinically depressed 62

patients and at-risk student samples. The current study aims to test the effectiveness of 63

cognitive control training as a preventive intervention in a remitted depressed sample, 64

exploring effects of cognitive control training on rumination and depressive symptomatology, 65

along with indicators of adaptive emotion regulation and functioning. 66

Methods/design: We present a double blind randomized controlled design. Remitted 67

depressed adults will complete 10 online sessions of a cognitive control training targeting 68

working memory functioning or a low cognitive load training (active control condition) over a 69

period of 14 days. Effects of training on primary outcome measures of rumination and 70

depressive symptomatology will be assessed pre-post training and at three months follow-up, 71

along with secondary outcome measure adaptive emotion regulation. Long-term effects of 72

cognitive control training on broader indicators of functioning will be assessed at three 73

months follow-up (secondary outcome measures). 74

Discussion: This study will provide information about the effectiveness of cognitive control 75

training for remitted depressed adults in reducing vulnerability for depression. Furthermore, 76

this study will address key questions concerning the mechanisms underlying the effects of 77

cognitive control training, will take into account the subjective experience of the patients 78

(including a self-report measure for cognitive functioning), and explore whether these effects 79

extend to broad measures of functioning such as Quality of Life and disability. 80

(4)

Trial registration: This study is registered with ClinicalTrials.Gov, number NCT02407652. 81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 89 90 91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98

(5)

Keywords: cognitive control, training, rumination, depression, remitted depressed, relapse 99

prevention, randomized controlled trial 100 101 102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 113 114 115 116 117

(6)

Background 118

Improving the effectiveness of psychotherapeutic interventions for depression forms an 119

important challenge for depression research. That is, patients who initially respond successful 120

to therapy, often show residual symptoms which increases the chance of recurrence of 121

depressive episodes. Moreover, existing treatments are less effective for chronic depression 122

[1] and not all depressive symptoms show an equal response to treatment [2]. For instance, 123

cognitive symptoms such as impaired executive- and working memory functioning and their 124

biological substrates often remain present although the patient is considered to be in remission 125

[e.g., 3, 4]. Importantly, it has been suggested that reduced cognitive functioning – i.e., 126

impaired regulation of working memory, or ‘cognitive control’ – is not merely a byproduct of 127

depression, but places remitted depressed (RMD) patients in a distinct vulnerable position for 128

recurrence of depression [5, 6]. 129

Indeed, the number of previous depressive episodes shows a negative correlation with 130

behavioral indices of cognitive control [7]. Furthermore, prospective studies suggest that self-131

reported cognitive control impairments [8] and their behavioral indices [9] predict the 132

development of future depressive symptomatology. Interestingly, impaired cognitive control 133

has typically been linked to maladaptive emotion regulation strategies such as rumination [9-134

11], an important cognitive vulnerability factor for depression [12]. Especially brooding – a 135

subtype of rumination that is characterized by a passive style of moody pondering – has 136

shown to predict the occurrence of future depressive symptomatology [13]. Importantly, 137

prospective studies indicate that the use of maladaptive emotion regulation strategies link 138

impaired cognitive control to the development of future depressive symptomatology in RMD 139

[14]. Thus, via maladaptive emotion regulation cognitive control impairments convey an 140

important risk for recurrent depression [but see 15]. Moreover, this mechanism is believed to 141

(7)

sustain and increase biological and cognitive vulnerability for recurrent depression [for a 142

review, see 6]. 143

In accordance with studies indicating plasticity of executive and working memory 144

functioning [16], these findings have led researchers to try to remediate cognitive control 145

impairments in depression using cognitive training tasks. In a pilot study, Siegle et al. [17] 146

demonstrated that combining treatment as usual (TAU) with a cognitive control training 147

(CCT) shows potential in reducing rumination as well as depressive symptomatology in a 148

limited MDD sample. The CCT existed of the adaptive Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task 149

[PASAT; 18] and Well’s Attention training [19]. Furthermore, Siegle et al. [20] have 150

extended these findings, showing long term beneficial effects of CCT by demonstrating a 151

reduced need for outpatient services at one year follow-up. Interestingly, whereas previous 152

studies have demonstrated the potential of a combined CCT approach [17, 20-22], other 153

authors have shown that the training component targeting working memory functioning (the 154

adaptive PASAT) might suffice to reduce brooding [23] and depressive symptomatology [24] 155

in MDD patients. 156

These first experimental findings are in line with existing conceptual frameworks 157

concerning the role of cognitive control and rumination in recurrent depression [6, 25], 158

suggesting that by remediating cognitive control impairments, one might decrease cognitive 159

vulnerability for future depression. Accordingly, Siegle et al. [20] have suggested that effects 160

of CCT on depressive symptomatology are preceded by changes in rumination. However, to 161

date no experimental study has directly tested this mediation effect. Furthermore, previous 162

studies have typically explored curative effects of CCT in MDD patients whereas only more 163

recently the preventive potential of CCT has been explored in student populations. For 164

instance, in a single session cognitive control manipulation, Cohen et al. [26] have 165

demonstrated that inducing cognitive control while processing negative information buffers 166

(8)

against negative effects of a subsequent rumination induction procedure (i.e., state rumination, 167

rumination-related sad mood). Moreover, training inhibition of emotional information has 168

shown to reduce rumination in at-risk students [27]. Interestingly, researchers have found that 169

the adaptive PASAT shows promise in reducing stress reactivity and rumination in response 170

to a lab stressor directly following training and a naturalistic stressor at one month follow-up 171

in an at-risk student sample [28]. Furthermore, decreased stress reactivity in confrontation 172

with a lab stressor predicted lower brooding levels following confrontation with naturalistic 173

stress (i.e., examination period). These findings suggest that CCT targeting working memory 174

functioning shows potential as a preventive intervention for depression. 175

Rational for the proposed study 176

Previous studies indicate that the effects of CCT are not limited to the mere reduction of 177

current depressive symptomatology in MDD patients, but might also extend to increasing 178

resilience in at-risk populations. However, several theoretical gaps remain to be addressed. 179

First, in order to fully explore the potential of CCT targeting working memory 180

functioning in reducing (cognitive) vulnerability for depression, a test of training effects in a 181

RMD sample would be desirable. That is, RMD patients form a high-risk group for 182

developing future depressive episodes [29] and prospective studies indicate that impaired 183

cognitive control forms an important vulnerability factor in RMD [14]. Second, from a 184

theoretical stance it would be interesting to explore the proposed mediational pathway from 185

effects of CCT on rumination to reduced future depressive symptomatology. Third, with the 186

exception of Siegle et al. [20] who explored effects of CCT on outpatient service use, 187

previous studies have limited their scope to exploring effects of CCT on rumination and 188

depressive symptomatology. We aim at extending previous findings by also exploring effects 189

of CCT on adaptive emotion regulation as well as broader indicators of (dis-)functioning such 190

(9)

as experienced disability, experienced remission from depression, and Quality of Life. 191

Furthermore, we are not only interested in change in behavior indices of cognitive control, but 192

also in the clinical experience of RMD patients concerning these cognitive factors (e.g., self-193

report measures of executive- and working memory functioning). Finally, in order to reduce 194

sources of bias in exploring the potential of CCT as a preventive intervention, a rigid 195

methodological approach – i.e., a double-blind randomized controlled trial (RCT) – is 196 required. 197 Method 198 Design 199

We present a 2 (Condition) x 3 (Time) double blind, randomized controlled design. 200

Adult RMD participants will be randomly allocated to either an online CCT intervention 201

targeting working memory functioning or a low cognitive load training (active control 202

condition). Both groups will perform 10 online training sessions over a period of 14 days, 203

flanked by pre- and post-training lab assessments. Participants will return to the lab for a final 204

assessment at three months follow-up (see Figure 1 for an overview of the design). This study 205

has been approved by the local ethical committee of the Faculty of Psychology and 206

Educational Sciences of Ghent University and was registered with ClinicalTrials.gov, number 207

NCT02407652. 208

Participants 209

Inclusion and exclusion criteria 210

To be eligible for participation to this study, participants aged between 23 and 65 should 211

report a history of depression (at least one prior depressive episode) and report stable (partial) 212

remission (≥ 6 months). Consequently, participants should not meet criteria for a current 213

(10)

depressive episode before starting training as assessed by the Mini-International 214

Neuropsychiatric Interview [MINI; 30]. However, they should meet the criteria for a previous 215

episode. The depressive episode should not have occurred in the context of a bipolar disorder. 216

Neither should the participant report a history of psychosis, excessive substance abuse, or 217

report experiencing cognitive impairments due to brain injury. A history of other comorbid 218

disorders is allowed – yet these should not lead to current impairments – in order to increase 219

the clinical relevance and validity of our study. Therapeutic maintenance contact (with a 220

frequency less than once per three weeks) and use of antidepressant medication is allowed and 221

will be registered. Importantly, antidepressant medication should be kept at a constant level 222

throughout the course of the study. 223

Recruitment 224

RMD participants will be recruited using advertisements in popular (online) magazines 225

and newspapers as well as flyers that were placed in 106 local drugstores (Ghent area). 226

Furthermore, drawing on an existing database, 23 potentially interested participants will be 227

contacted based on their interest in a prior prospective study of our lab (at that point, all of 228

these participants had given their permission to be re-contacted in case a related study was 229

planned). After having expressed interest in this study (i.e., by phone or e-mail), participants 230

will be contacted by phone to provide further information and to screen eligibility based on a 231

selection of relevant questions of the MINI screening version. To screen whether participants 232

show a history of depression, we will ask questions concerning current and previous 233

depressive symptoms, and collect information concerning the amount of episodes and past as 234

well as current treatment. Furthermore, we will check whether (professional or similar) 235

activities were resumed following the last depressive episode. If the participant seems eligible 236

and is interested in participating in the study, he or she will be invited to the lab for a 237

structured clinical interview (MINI). In the lab, the MINI screening version will be used to 238

(11)

check for indicators of currently present comorbid disorders and – if this proves to be 239

necessary – will be followed by the corresponding parts of the MINI interview to allow to 240

control for presence of comorbid disorders. The parts related to MDD will be fully assessed to 241

assure that participants do not meet the criteria for MDD before entering the study. Meeting 242

eligibility criteria will allow the participant to enroll in the study, starting immediately with 243

randomization over one of both conditions and the baseline assessment. 244

Measures 245

Primary outcome measures 246

Rumination and depressive symptomatology form our primary outcome measures. 247

Rumination will be assessed using the Ruminative Response Scale [RRS; 13, 31]. This 22-248

item questionnaire provides a total rumination score (range: 22 – 88), as well as Brooding and 249

Reflection subscale scores (range: 5 – 20). Brooding is characterized by a passive style of 250

moody pondering and is the most maladaptive form of depressive rumination [13, 32]. 251

Depressive symptomatology will be assessed using the 21-item (range: 0 – 63) Beck 252

Depression Inventory [BDI-II; 33, 34]. Both primary outcome measures exhibit adequate 253

psychometric properties [13, 33, 34]. 254

Secondary outcome measures 255

Adaptive emotion regulation and indicators of RMD functioning will be our secondary 256

outcome measures. Adaptive emotion regulation will be assessed using the five adaptive 257

subscales of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire [CERQ; 35]. The four less 258

adaptive strategies (self-blame, rumination, catastrophizing, and blaming others) can be used 259

as a second, alternative measure for maladaptive emotion regulation (range: 4 – 20). 260

Functioning will be operationalized by indices of disability, quality of life, resilience, 261

and remission from depression. Disability will be assessed using the self-report version of the 262

World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule 2.0 [WHODAS 2.0; 36], 263

(12)

consisting of 36 items. This measure is based on the conceptual framework of the 264

International Classification of Functioning (ICF) and provides indicators of overall 265

functioning and six specific domains of functioning (Cognition, Mobility, Self-care, Getting 266

along, Life activities, and Participation). Furthermore, the questionnaire provides an estimate 267

of the amount of days in the past month during which the difficulties (a) were present, (b) 268

prevented the participant from performing his/her daily activities or work, or (c) formed a 269

source of reduced functioning. Quality of life will be assessed using the depression-specific 270

34-item (range: 0 – 34) Quality of Life in Depression Scale [QLDS; 37, 38]. Resilience will 271

be assessed using the Resilience Scale [RS; 39, 40]. We will use the 25-item version of the RS 272

using four point Likert-scales (range: 25 – 100). Finally, self-reported remission from 273

depression will be assessed using the 41-item Remission of Depression Questionnaire [RDQ; 274

41, 42] for which a high score is indicative for more psychopathology (range: 0 – 82). 275

Manipulation check, training process and cognitive transfer measures 276

As a manipulation check and process measure, training task performance will be 277

assessed in both conditions using median inter stimulus interval (ISI) levels per training 278

session. Furthermore, as a process measure of effects of completing an online training session, 279

mood (‘energetic’, ‘tense’, ‘frustrated’, ‘sad’, ‘happy’) will be assessed using visual analogue 280

scales (VAS; 1 – 100). The extent to which participants have experienced negative thoughts 281

and stress throughout the training session will also be assessed using VAS, along with 282

experienced task competence (‘During the task I felt as if I was doing great’). It has been 283

suggested that training cognitive control in a frustrating task-context – and thus, eliciting low 284

levels of negative affect while training – contributes to the beneficial effects of CCT [28]. 285

These process measures allow to explore the mechanism underlying CCT. Furthermore, we 286

will use the Credibility/Expectancy Questionnaire [CEQ; 43, 44] to check for baseline group 287

differences in treatment credibility/expectancy and to check for successful blinding of 288

(13)

participants (post-training). Moreover, we will monitor intake of antidepressants and other 289

forms of therapy as well as stressful life events that might influence our findings. For the 290

latter, we will use the List of Threatening Experiences [LTE; 45, 46]. 291

Close transfer to cognitive control will be assessed using the non-adaptive PASAT [18]. 292

During this task, participants are presented with a practice phase consisting of 10 trials, 293

followed by a test phase, consisting of three blocks with increasing difficulty (ISI block 1 = 294

3000 ms; ISI block 2 = 2000 ms; ISI block 3 = 1500 ms), each containing 60 trials. 295

Furthermore, we will include the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function Adult 296

version [BRIEF-A; 47] as a self-report measure to assess experienced cognitive control. This 297

75-item self-report questionnaire provides several estimates of executive and working 298

memory functioning (e.g., inhibition, shifting, emotional control, working memory). 299

Interventions 300

Participants will either be subjected to an online CCT targeting working memory 301

functioning (the adaptive PASAT) or a closely matched low cognitive load training. This 302

allows to rule out motivational aspects of performing an adaptive computer task online. The 303

tasks will be presented in-browser, using a Millisecond software Web license. Both training 304

groups will be asked to perform 10 sessions of 400 trials (which takes 20 min per session at 305

an average inter stimulus interval (ISI) of 3000 ms), providing similar learning experiences in 306

both conditions. Prior to training, both groups will receive oral and written psycho-education 307

concerning cognitive control training [based on the protocol of 17] in order to enhance task 308

engagement. This is important as previous work indicates that task engagement forms an 309

important predictor of response to CCT [20]. Importantly, no explicit information will be 310

given about the to be expected results. Furthermore, participants will receive an automated 311

(14)

text message on a daily basis to prevent attrition during the training period [using 312

SurveySignal software; 48]. 313

Cognitive control training condition 314

We will use an adaptive version of the PASAT [17, 18] to train participants’ cognitive 315

control in the CCT condition. Participants will be presented with a continuous stream of 316

auditory digits (1 – 9) and are instructed to immediately respond to the sum of the last two 317

heard digits by clicking the corresponding response buttons (1 – 18). The speed of number 318

presentation is adapted based on participants’ performance in order to train cognitive control 319

in a frustrating task context. Participants begin each session with a 3000 ms ISI, which is 320

reduced by 100 ms following every four consecutive correct responses, increasing task 321

difficulty. Following every four incorrect responses the ISI increases with 100 ms, reducing 322

task difficulty. Throughout each session participants are presented with their current ISI and 323

amount of consecutive correct and incorrect responses. Participants’ responses and response 324

times are being measured. In line with previous training studies the median ISI per session 325

will be used as an indicator of ones performance during the training sessions. 326

Active control condition 327

In the active control condition, participants will be presented with a low cognitive load 328

version of the adaptive PASAT. This training task shows high resemblance to the adaptive 329

PASAT concerning stimuli, responses, modification of task difficulty, and evaluation of 330

session performance. However, in this low cognitive load version of the adaptive PASAT 331

participants are instructed to immediately respond to the last heard digit instead of mentally 332

manipulating the content in working memory (i.e., instead of responding to the sum of the last 333

two heard digits as in the CCT condition). To better resemble the response options of the 334

(15)

adaptive PASAT, participants in the active control condition are presented auditory stimuli 335 ranging from 1 – 18. 336 Sample size 337

We are the first to explore effects of CCT targeting working memory functioning in a 338

RMD sample which makes it impossible to provide an exact estimate of effect size for the 339

main outcome measure in this sample. However, previous work on MDD patients has yielded 340

an effect size of ɳ² = .19 for brooding [20], whereas work with at-risk undergraduate students 341

revealed an effect size of ɳ² = .11 in confrontation with naturalistic stress [28]. Given that this 342

study will use an at-risk sample (RMD), we will base estimations of sample size on the latter 343

effect size. In order to be able to detect a similar effect over two time points with α = .05 and 344

1-β = .80, the total sample size should at least be 68 (n CCT = 34, n active control = 34). We 345

will stop recruiting once 68 participants have entered the training phase. 346

Randomization 347

Upon entering the study, participants will receive a sealed envelope containing an 348

exterior subject number that will be used for registration purposes during the assessment 349

sessions in the lab (baseline, post-training, and follow-up). The envelope will contain a 350

training manual, an URL that directs participants to the online training task, and a personal 351

training task identification code that should be used while performing the ten online training 352

sessions at home. Prior to the study, an independent researcher will randomly link the training 353

task identification codes to the subject numbers that will be used in the lab using an 354

automated randomization program (RandList; randomisation.eu). This researcher will prepare 355

the envelopes and keep a list of the linked subject numbers and training session identification 356

codes in a locked closet at the office and a copy at home for safe keeping. Based on the 357

(16)

training task identification codes, participants will either perform the CCT or low cognitive 358 load training. 359 Blinding 360

We present a double blind RCT design. Prior to the randomization procedure, the 361

independent researcher will reset the online training task so that even-numbered training task 362

identification codes will redirect the participants to one condition (CCT or active control), 363

whereas odd-numbered training task identification codes will redirect the participants to the 364

other condition. The researchers of this study will not be aware of the training task 365

identification codes (these are randomly generated and presented in a sealed envelope) or the 366

link between even- or odd-numbered identification codes and training condition. Furthermore, 367

participants will be instructed not to share details concerning the content of the training task 368

or the personal training task identification code with the researchers. 369

During data-analysis, the researchers will remain blind of training task condition by 370

separating (a) analysis of training task performance and process measures (based on even- or 371

odd-numbered training task identification codes) from (b) analysis of training effects on the 372

outcome measures. Concerning the latter, the independent researcher will provide the 373

researchers with a list grouping the subject numbers – used during the lab sessions – in two 374

non-informative conditions following completion of data-collection. Importantly, at this point 375

(lab) subject numbers will not be linked to the personal training task identification codes. This 376

allows blind evaluation of training effects. The blinding will only be broken for the more 377

explorative analyses linking training task process measures with the outcome measures. 378

Furthermore, we will use the CEQ-data to check for successful blinding of participants. 379

Analysis 380

(17)

In line with Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials [CONSORT; 49], we will use 381

intention-to-treat (ITT) analysis to test effects of CCT on primary and secondary measures 382

post-training and at follow-up. Missing data will be handled using the Last-Observation-383

Carried-Forward (LOCF) method. Effects of CCT will be tested using Repeated Measures, 384

analysis of variance (ANOVA), or covariance (ANCOVA) with follow-up t-tests. Exploratory 385

analysis will take into account potential moderators of training effects such as variability in 386

baseline depressive symptomatology and cognitive control. ITT might not necessarily apply 387

to the exploratory analyses such as analysis of process measures of training. As secondary 388

analysis, we will also perform completers-only analyses. Explorative within-group mediation 389

analysis will be performed using a stepwise regression approach [50] and the Preacher and 390

Hayes [51] bootstrapping method. 391

Procedure 392

Eligibility will be assessed by a clinical psychologist. Participants will first undergo a 393

telephone screening to assess eligibility (see Figure 1). Second, potential participants will be 394

invited to the lab where eligibility will be further assessed using the MINI. After giving 395

informed consent, eligible participants will be randomized and the baseline assessment will 396

take place (see Table 1). At baseline (Time 1), the behavior measure for cognitive control will 397

be completed followed by the self-report measure for cognitive control (BRIEF-A). Next, 398

participants will complete the other self-report questionnaires (primary outcome measures: 399

BDI-II, RRS; secondary outcome measures: CERQ, QLDS, WHODAS 2.0, RS, RDQ) and 400

will receive psycho-education concerning cognitive training for depression and practical 401

information about the intervention. Participants will be instructed to complete 10 training 402

sessions during a period of 14 days following the baseline assessment and will be asked to 403

perform only one session a day. At the end of the baseline assessment session, the CEQ will 404

be administered and participants’ telephone number will be registered using SurveySignal 405

(18)

software. During the 14-days period of online training, participants will receive a daily 406

automated text message reminding them to complete the training. Each training session will 407

consist of 400 trials of the adaptive PASAT or a low cognitive load training and will include 408

assessments of affect and worrying throughout and following training. Upon completing 409

training, participants will return to the lab for the post-training assessment (Time 2) during 410

which direct effects of CCT on cognitive control and the primary outcome measures and 411

adaptive emotion regulation will be assessed. At the end of the post-training session the CEQ 412

will be administered to rule out group differences in expectancy and credibility of the 413

intervention. Finally, participants will return to the lab at three months follow-up (Time 3) 414

during which long-term effects on cognitive control and the primary and secondary outcome 415

measures (including indicators of functioning) will be assessed. At each time point we will 416

assess stressful life events (LTE), intake of antidepressants and other forms of therapy. Upon 417

completion of the follow-up assessment session, participants will receive reimbursement 418

(€75) followed by a partial written and oral debriefing. Importantly, participants will only 419

receive feedback concerning their condition following processing of the data of the total 420

sample. If CCT shows to have beneficial effects in RMD, participants from the active control 421

condition will be offered the chance to perform the CCT online. 422

Discussion 423

Prevention of recurrent depression is an important target for interventions. Previous 424

findings indicate that CCT shows potential in reducing depressive symptomatology and 425

rumination in MDD as well as cognitive vulnerability in at-risk undergraduate students. To 426

test the potential of CCT as a preventive intervention for depression, the present study aims to 427

test the effectiveness of CCT in a RMD sample. We will test whether CCT targeting working 428

memory functioning – as compared to a low cognitive load training – can be used to reduce 429

vulnerability for depression over a 3.5 months period. We hypothesize that CCT will have 430

(19)

beneficial effects on primary outcome measures depressive rumination (i.e., brooding) and 431

depressive symptomatology and hope to see these findings extend to adaptive emotion 432

regulation and long-term functioning (secondary outcome measures). 433

This double blind RCT study forms a first test of the potential of CCT as a preventive 434

intervention for depression in RMD. Furthermore, these findings will be informative to the 435

literature as several exploratory questions will be addressed in order to further elucidate the 436

role of cognitive control in vulnerability for depression. First, we will explore whether effects 437

of CCT on depressive symptomatology are mediated by rumination. Second, we will explore 438

whether effects of CCT extend to measures of adaptive emotion regulation and indices of 439

functioning such as quality of life and disability. Third, in order to further elucidate the 440

mechanisms involved during CCT, we could explore how process measures of CCT relate to 441

effects of training. 442

Overall, this study will further enhance the knowledge on the role of cognitive control 443

in emotion regulation and vulnerability for depression. This study forms a first step in testing 444

the effectiveness of CCT targeting working memory functioning as a preventive intervention 445

for (recurrent) depression. If these first results show to be promising, future work should 446

focus on replicating the effects of CCT and exploring how this preventive intervention could 447

best be implemented. 448

Trial status 449

We are currently recruiting RMD patients for this study. The study entered the data 450

collection phase in April 2015. 451

452 453

(20)

List of abbreviations 454

ANCOVA: analysis of covariance 455

ANOVA: analysis of variance 456

BDI-II: Beck Depression Inventory 2nd edition 457

BRIEF-A: Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function Adult version 458

CCT: cognitive control training 459

CEQ: Credibility/Expectancy Questionnaire 460

CERQ: Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire 461

CONSORT: Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials 462

ICF: International Classification of Functioning 463

ISI: inter stimulus interval 464

ITT: intention-to-treat 465

LOCF: Last-Observation-Carried-Forward 466

LTE: List of Threatening Experiences 467

MDD: major depressive disorder 468

PASAT: Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task 469

QLDS: Quality of Life in Depression Scale 470

RCT: randomized controlled trial 471

RDQ: Remission of Depression Questionnaire 472

RMD: remitted depressed 473

RRS: Ruminative Response Scale 474

RS: Resilience Scale 475

WHODAS: World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule 2.0 476

477

(21)

Competing interests 479

The authors of this article report no competing interests. 480

Author’s contributions 481

First and last authors were equally involved in the development of the design. First 482

drafts of the manuscript were made by KH and further adapted by EHWK. LF and JB are 483

involved in the project as master students. All authors agreed upon the final version of the 484

manuscript. 485

Acknowledgments 486

This research will be supported by a Special Research Fund (BOF) of Ghent University 487

awarded to Kristof Hoorelbeke (B/13808/01). A license to use the WHODAS 2.0 for the 488

purpose of this study was achieved by the WHO (22-10-2014). 489 490 491 492 493 494 495 496 497 498 499 500 501 502

(22)

References 503

1. Cuijpers P, Andersson G, Donker T, van Straten A: Psychological treatment of 504

depression: Results of a series of meta-analyses. Nord J Psychiatr 2011, 65(6):354-505

364. 506

2. Millan MJ, Agid Y, Brüne M, Bullmore ET, Carter CS, Clayton NS, Connor R, Davis 507

S, Deakin B, DeRubeis RJ et al: Cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: 508

characteristics, causes and the quest for improved therapy. Nature Reviews 2012, 509

11:141-168. 510

3. Vanderhasselt M-A, De Raedt R: Impairments in cognitive control persist during 511

remission from depression and are related to the number of past episodes: An 512

event related potentials study. Biol Psychol 2009, 81(3):169-176. 513

4. Xu GY, Lin KG, Rao DP, Dang YM, Ouyang HY, Guo YB, Ma JX, Chen JC: 514

Neuropsychological performance in bipolar I, bipolar II and unipolar depression 515

patients: A longitudinal, naturalistic study. J Affect Disord 2012, 136(3):328-339. 516

5. Gotlib IH, Joormann J: Cognition and Depression: Current Status and Future 517

Directions. In: Annu Rev Clin Psychol, Vol 6. Volume 6, edn. Edited by Nolen-518

Hoeksema S, Cannon TD, Widiger T. Palo Alto: Annual Reviews; 2010: 285-312. 519

6. De Raedt R, Koster EHW: Understanding vulnerability for depression from a 520

cognitive neuroscience perspective: A reappraisal of attentional factors and a 521

new conceptual framework. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 2010, 10(1):50-70. 522

7. Harvey PO, Le Bastard G, Pochon JB, Levy R, Allilaire JF, Dubois B, Fossti P: 523

Executive functions and updating of the contents of working memory in unipolar 524

depression. J Psychiatr Res 2004, 38(6):567-576. 525

(23)

8. Letkiewicz AM, Miller GA, Crocker LD, Warren SL, Infantolino ZP, Mimnaugh KJ, 526

Heller W: Executive function deficits in daily life prospectively predict increases 527

in depressive symptoms. Cogn Ther Res 2014, 38(6):612-620. 528

9. Zetsche U, Joormann J: Components of interference control predict depressive 529

symptoms and rumination cross-sectionally and at six months follow-up. J Behav 530

Ther Exp Psychiatry 2011, 42(1):65-73. 531

10. De Lissnyder E, Koster EHW, Goubert L, Onraedt T, Vanderhasselt MA, De Raedt R: 532

Cognitive control moderates the association between stress and rumination. J 533

Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 2012, 43(1):519-525. 534

11. Joormann J, Gotlib IH: Emotion regulation in depression: Relation to cognitive 535

inhibition. Cogn Emot 2010, 24(2):281-298. 536

12. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Wisco BE, Lyubomirsky S: Rethinking rumination. Perspect 537

Psychol Sci 2008, 3(5):400-424. 538

13. Treynor W, Gonzalez R, Nolen-Hoeksema S: Rumination reconsidered: A 539

psychometric analysis. Cogn Ther Res 2003, 27:247-259. 540

14. Demeyer I, De Lissnyder E, Koster EHW, De Raedt R: Rumination mediates the 541

relationship between impaired cognitive control for emotional information and 542

depressive symptoms: A prospective study in remitted depressed adults. Behav 543

Res Ther 2012, 50(5):292-297. 544

15. Aker M, Harmer C, Landro NI: More rumination and less effective emotion 545

regulation in previously depressed women with preserved executive functions. 546

BMC Psychiatry 2014, 14:10. 547

16. Klingberg T: Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends Cogn Sci 2010, 548

14(7):317-324. 549

(24)

17. Siegle GJ, Ghinassi F, Thase ME: Neurobehavioral therapies in the 21st century: 550

Summary of an emerging field and an extended example of cognitive control 551

training for depression. Cogn Ther Res 2007, 31(2):235-262. 552

18. Gronwall D: Paced Auditory Serial-Addition Task: A measure of recovery from 553

concussion. Percept Mot Skills 1977, 44:367-373. 554

19. Wells A: Emotional disorders and metacognition innovative Cognitive Therapy. 555

New York: Wiley; 2000. 556

20. Siegle GJ, Price RB, Jones NP, Ghinassi F, Painter T, Thase ME: You gotta work at 557

it: Pupillary indices of task focus are prognostic for response to a neurocognitive 558

intervention for rumination in depression. Clinical Psychological Science 2014, 559

2:455-471. 560

21. Calkins AW, McMorran KE, Siegle GJ, Otto MW: The effects of computerized 561

cognitive control training on community adults with depressed mood. Behav 562

Cognit Psychther in press. 563

22. Segrave RA, Arnold S, Hoy K, Fitzgerald PB: Concurrent cognitive control 564

training augments the antidepressant efficacy of tDCS: A pilot study. Brain Stimul 565

2014, 7(2):325-331. 566

23. Vanderhasselt M-A, De Raedt R, Namur V, Lotufo PA, Bensenor IM, Boggio PS, 567

Brunoni AR: Transcranial electric stimulation and neurocognitive training in 568

clinically depressed patients: A pilot study of the effects on rumination. Prog 569

Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry in press, 57:93-99. 570

24. Brunoni AR, Boggio PS, De Raedt R, Bensenor IM, Lotufo PA, Namur V, Valiengo 571

LCL, Vanderhasselt MA: Cognitive control therapy and transcranial direct 572

current stimulation for depression: A randomized, double-blinded, controlled 573

trial. J Affect Disord 2014, 162:43-49. 574

(25)

25. Joormann J, D'Avanzato C: Emotion regulation in depression: Examining the role 575

of cognitive processes. 2010 2010, 24:913-939. 576

26. Cohen N, Mor N, Henik A: Linking executive control and emotional response: A 577

training procedure to reduce rumination. Clinical Psychological Science in press. 578

27. Daches S, Mor N: Training ruminators to inhibit negative information: A 579

preliminary report. Cogn Ther Res 2014, 38(2):160-171. 580

28. Hoorelbeke K, Koster EHW, Vanderhasselt MA, Callewaert S, Demeyer I: The 581

influence of cognitive control training on stress reactivity and rumination in 582

response to a lab stressor and naturalistic stress. Behav Res Ther in press. 583

29. Burcusa SL, Iacono WG: Risk for recurrence in depression. Clin Psychol Rev 2007, 584

27(8):959-985. 585

30. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, Herqueta T, 586

Baker R, Dunbar GC: The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI): 587

The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview 588

for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry 1989, 59:22-33. 589

31. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Morrow J: A prospective study of depression and 590

posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster - The 1989 Loma-Prieta 591

earthquake. J Pers Soc Psychol 1991, 61(1):115-121. 592

32. Joormann J, Dkane M, Gotlib IH: Adaptive and maladaptive components of 593

rumination? Diagnostic specificity and relation to depressive biases. Behavior 594

Therapy 2006, 37:269-280. 595

33. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK: Manual for Beck Depression Inventory II. San 596

Antonio, Texas: Psychological Corporation; 1996. 597

34. Van der Does AJW: BDI-II-NL. Handleiding: De Nederlandse versie van de Beck 598

Depression Inventory - 2nd edition. Lisse: Harcourt Test Publishers; 2002. 599

(26)

35. Garnefski N, Kraaij V, Spinhoven P: Negative life events, cognitive emotion 600

regulation and emotional problems. Pers Individ Differ 2001, 30:1311-1327. 601

36. Üstün T, Kostanjsek N, Chatterji S, Rehm J: Measuring health and disability: 602

Manual for WHO Disability Assessment Schedule (WHODAS 2.0). 2010:World 603

Health Organization, Switzerland: Geneva. 604

37. Hunt SM, McKenna SP: The QLDS: A scale for the measurement of quality of life 605

in depression. Health Policy 1992, 22:307-319. 606

38. Tuynman-Qua H, de Jonghe F, McKenna SP: Quality of Life in Depression Scale 607

(QLDS). Development, reliability, validity, responsiveness and application. Eur 608

Psychiatry 1997, 12:199-202. 609

39. Wagnild GM, Young HM: Development and psychometric evaluation of the 610

resilience scale. J Nurs Meas 1993, 1:165-178. 611

40. Portzky M: RS-nl: Resilience Scale - Nederlandse versie. Amsterdam: Harcourt Test 612

Publishers; 2008. 613

41. Zimmerman M, Martinez JH, Attiullah N, Friedman M, Toba C, Boerescu DA, 614

Ragheb M: A new type of scale for determining remission from depression: The 615

Remission from Depression Questionnaire. J Psychiatr Res 2013, 47:78-82. 616

42. Peeters F, Nicolson N, Wichers M, Hacker P: Remission of Depression 617

Questionnaire - Dutch translation (RDQ-NL). 2013:Unpublished document. 618

43. Devilly GJ, Borkovec TD: Psychometric properties of the credibility/expectancy 619

questionnaire. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 2000, 31:73-86. 620

44. Godfrin K, Goeleven E, Schoof P: Dutch translation of the Credibility/Expectancy 621

Questionnaire (CEQ-NL). 2004:Unpublished document. 622

45. Rosmalen JGM, Bos EH, de Jonge P: Validation of the Long-term Difficulties 623

Inventory (LDI) and the List of Threatening Experiences (LTE) as measures of 624

(27)

stress in epidemiological population-based cohort studies. Psychol Med 2012, 625

42:2599-2608. 626

46. Brugha TS, Cragg D: The List of Threatening Experiences: the reliability and 627

validity of a brief life events questionnaire. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1990, 82:77-81. 628

47. Roth RM, Lance CE, Isquith PK, Fischer AS, Giancola PR: Confirmatory factor 629

analysis of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Adult version in 630

healthy adults and application to attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. 631

Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 2013, 28:425-434. 632

48. Hofmann W, Patel PV: SurveySignal: A convenient solution for experience 633

sampling research using participants' own smartphones. Social Science Computer 634

Review in press. 635

49. Moher D, Hopewell S, Schulz KF, Montori V, Gotzsche PC, Devereaux PJ, Elbourne 636

D, Egger M, Altman DG: CONSORT 2010 Explanation and elaboration: Updated 637

guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. British Medical Journal 638

2010:340:328. 639

50. Baron RM, Kenny DA: The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social 640

psychological research - Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J 641

Pers Soc Psychol 1986, 51(6):1173-1182. 642

51. Preacher KJ, Hayes AF: SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects 643

in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and 644 Computers 2004, 36(4):717-731. 645 646 647 648

(28)

Table 1 Schedule of measures Instrument Telephone screening Baseline Online training Post-training (2 weeks) Follow-up (3 months)

Inclusion criteria interview X

MINI Screen and structured interview X

Process measures of affect during training X

Process measures of training task performance (ISI) X

Credibility and expectancy of treatment (CEQ) X X

Stressful life events (LTE) X X X

Self-reported use of antidepressants and other forms of therapy

X X X

Cognitive control (non-adaptive PASAT / transfer task) X X X

Self-reported cognitive control (BRIEF-A) X X X

Depressive symptomatology (BDI-II) X X X

Depressive rumination (RRS) X X X

Cognitive emotion regulation (CERQ) X X X

Quality of Life (QLDS) X X

Disability (WHODAS 2.0) X X

Resilience (RS) X X

(29)

References

Related documents

We selected four main crater units, including the LTDs, a small outcrop of dark mesa material, the cratered plains material outside of the trough, and the low crater density

orientation of the long axis of proliferative chondrocytes shows that θ cell for Vangl2 expression is uniformly distributed from 0-90 ° , whereas uninfected neighboring cells

We present results to support the hypothesis that greater selection for gene expression robustness against temperature perturbation occurs in the natural strains compared

Utilizing a decision support system is a proactive way to use data to manage, operate, and evaluate educational institute in a better way. Depending on the quality and

The proposed weighted fuzzy method has the lower delay due to monitor and data integrity is based on the generated weighted value instead of the continuous monitor method used

In this paper, we focus on decreasing the computational complexity required for ME process by applying both Adaptive Block Size Phase Correlation Motion Estimation

activities. The revenue portion is a linear function of throughput: the number of trains served by the railroad per unit time multiplied by a destination speeific railroad charge.

"Much of Enron's reputation as a world-class innovator comes from the creation of the gas bank and Enron's later efforts to duplicate that success in related markets, most