• No results found

Goal Formulation based on Communicative Principles

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2020

Share "Goal Formulation based on Communicative Principles"

Copied!
6
0
0

Loading.... (view fulltext now)

Full text

(1)

G o a l F o r m u l a t i o n b a s e d o n C o m m u n i c a t i v e

P r i n c i p l e s

K r i s t i i n a J o k i n e n *

C o m p u t a t i o n a l L i n g u i s t i c s L a b o r a t o r y G r a d u a t e S c h o o l o f I n f o r m a t i o n S c i e n c e N a r a I n s t i t u t e o f S c i e n c e a n d T e c h n o l o g y

8 9 1 6 - 5 T a k a y a m a , I k o m a , N a r a

6 3 0 - 0 1 J A P A N k r i s ~ i s . a i s t - n a r a . a c . j p

Abstract

The p a p e r presents the Constructive Dialogue Model as a new approach to formulate system goals in intelligent di- alogue systems. The departure point is in general communicative principles which constrain cooperative and coher- ent communication. Dialogue partici- pants are engaged in a cooperative task whereby a model of the joint purpose is constructed. Contributions are planned as reactions to the changing context, and no dialogue g r a m m a r is needed. Also speech act classification is aban- doned, in favour of contextual reasoning and rationality considerations.

1 Introduction

Two general approaches can be distinguished in dialogue management: the structural approach, which uses a dialogue g r a m m a r to capture regu- larities of the dialogue in terms of exchanges and moves (Bilange, 1992; Cawsey, 1993; Grosz and Sidner, 1986), and the intention-based approach, which classifies the speaker's beliefs and intentions into speech acts, and uses planning operators to describe t h e m (Appel% 1985; Allen and Perrault, 1980; Bunt et al., 1984). Both regard natural lan- guage as purposeful behaviour, but differ in how this behaviour is to be described. T h e former sees dialogues as products and compiles participants' beliefs and intentions into a predefined dialogue structure, whereas the latter focusses on the par- ticipants' goals, and hides the structure in the re- lations between acts which contain appropriately chosen sets of beliefs and intentions as their pre- conditions and effects.

We will not go into detailed evaluation of the approaches, see e.g. (Jokinen, 1994), but draw at- tention to three aspects of dialogues which have

*I am grateful to Yuji Matsumoto for providing an excellent resem'ch environment during my JSPS Post- doctoral Fellowship, and Graham Wilcock for helpful discussions.

not been properly addressed before, although widely acknowledged in literature, and i m p o r t a n t in building robust Natural Language interfaces:

1. dialogue is a collaborative process and its structure is recognised by external observa- tion, not prescribed as an internal constraint of dialogue m a n a g e m e n t (Sacks et al., 1974; Clark and Wilkes-Gibbs, 1990),

2. the speakers' beliefs and intentions in a given dialogue situation are various, and cannot all be checked when deciding oil the next re- sponse (Cherniak, 1986),

3. communicative acts are part of social activ- ity, constrained by normative obligations of rational agency (Allwood, 1976).

We discuss these aspects from the point of view of cooperative goal formulation and present the Constructive Dialogue Model as a new approach to plan system responses. Our departure point is in general conmmnicative principles which con- strain cooperative and coherent communication, and radical steps are taken in two respects: the dialogue g r a m m a r is abandoned as an infeasible way to describe dialogues, and also speech act recognition is abandoned as a redundant labelling of intention configurations. The first step means t h a t the structure is not built according to struc- turing rules, but emerges from local coherence as the dialogue goes on. T h e second step means that beliefs and intentions are dealt with by reasoning a b o u t the utterance context and communicative constraints instead of speech act types. T h e deci- sion about what to say next falls out as a result of the agent complying with the communicative prin- ciples which refer to the agent's rationality, sire cerity, motivation and consideration. Combined with contextual knowledge, they account for the acceptability of different alternative responses.

(2)

2

Constructive Dialogue

M o d e l

2.1 R a t i o n a l , ( : ( ) o p e r a t i v e w a y t o r e a c t

Rational agents try to follow the principles of Ideal Cooperation (Allwood, 1976) in comimmication: (1) assume a joint l)urpose, (2) show cognitive consideration (epistemic rationality regarding ap- propriate ways to react) and ethical consideration (intention to react in a way t h a t does not prevent the p a r t n e r fi'om fiflfilling her goals), and (3) trust, t h a t the partner is acting according to the same principles. Ideal cooperation does not mean t h a t the agents always react in the way the p a r t n e r in- tended to evoke, but rather, it sets the normality assumptions for the way the agents would behave if no disturbing factors were present. As (Gal- liers, 1989) points out, conflict resolution forms an i m p o r t a n t part of h u m a n conmmnication, and if systems are always ready to adopt the user's role, they becolne rigid and unrealistic. However, if the conflict becomes so serious t h a t it makes any cooperation impossible, communication will break down as well. Rational agents thus try to conlnlu- nicate so as to conforln to the shared assumptions a b o u t operationally appropriate and ethically ac- ceptable acts in a given situation (Jokinen, 11995). Empirical dialogue research has emphasised col- laborative nature of dialogues (Sacks et al., 1974; Clark and Wilkes-Gibbs, 1990). Also computa- tionally oriented dialogue studies show t h a t the users express themselves vaguely and continue with follow-up questions (Moore and Paris, 1993), and o u r c o r p u s 1 supports the view t h a t even sin> ple information seeking dialogues resemble nego- tiations rather than straightforward question-ans- wer sequences. Based on these obeservations and the theoretical assumptions of Ideal Cooperation, we distinguished the main factors in rational, co- operative dialogue m a n a g e m e n t as follows:

Surface form Expressive intentions Declarative express a belief:

want (Sp ,know(He ,P) )

Interrogative desire for information: want (Sp,know(Sp, P) )

h n p e r a t i v e desire for action:

want (Sp,do(ge,P)) Exclamative express action:

want (Sp, do (Sp,P))

whereas the information seeker is not ex- pected to teach the information provider how to look for the information. The roles can be further difl'erentiated with respect to social factors such as acquaintance of the addressee and fornlality of the situation.

C o m m u n i c a t i v e o b l i g a t i o n s . Social, nor- mative requirements t h a t concern the agent's sincerit'9 (exchange information which is true or for which evidence can be provided), mo- tivation (exchange information which is re- lated to one's goals and strategies), and con- sideration (exchange information which the. p a r t n e r is able to deal with).

• T a s k . Gives rise to communication. Task goals are planned to complete a real workt task (rent a car, book ~ flight, repair a pump) but because of uneven distribution of knowl- edge, the agents usually need to collaborate to achieve the goal, and thus formulate com- ,nunicative goals to obtain missing informa- tion, el. (Guilm, 1994).

B. C o m m m f i c a t i v e act:

• E x p r e s s i v e a n d e v o c a t i v e a t t i t u d e s . To distinguish between the effects of an utter- ance and the intentions behind it, Austin's concept of illocution is split up into two: ez- pvession of the speaker's a t t i t u d e and evoca- tion of a reaction in the partner; perlocution corresponds to what is actually achieved by the act: the evoked respoT~se, cf. (Allwood, 1976). Expression m a y differ fl'om evoca- tion (irony, indirectness), aud the evoked response fi'om the evocative intentions (the agent requests ilfformation t h a t the partner cannot or does not want to disclose; the agent fa.ils to fi'ighten the partner becmme this has guessed the agent's malicious intentions).

E v o c a t i v e i n t e n t i o n s

share the belief:

want (Sp, want (He, know(He, P) ) ) provide the desired information:

want (Sp, want (He, know (Sp, P) ) )

(provide) action:

want (Sp, want (He, do (He, P) ) )

attend to the action:

want (Sp, want (He, do (Sp, P) ) )

Figure 1: Conventional association of expressive and evocative intentions with surface form, modified fi'om (Allwood, 1992).

A . C o m m u n i c a t i v e s i t u a t i o n : C. C o m m u n i c a t i v e c o n t e x t :

• R o l e . Characterised by global communica- • E x p e c t a t i o n s . Evocative intentions put live rights and obligations of the agents. E.g.

the information provider is expected to give information which is relevant for the task,

1The corpus was collected by the Wizard-of-Oz technique with users trying to find information on car- [tire companies and restaurants in a particular area, and is reported in (Nivre, 1992).

(3)

also the right to pursue the goal until it is achieved or not relevant anymore. She also has the right to expect the p a r t n e r to collab- orate or at least not prevent the agent from achieving her goal.

• U n f u l f i l l e d g o a l s . If the expressive attitu- des of the partner's response m a t c h the evocative intentions of the agent's contribu- tion, the communicative goal of the agent's contribution is fulfilled. An unfulfilled goal is pushed forward or stored for later processing. When the agent has the right to take the ini- tiative, a previously unfulfilled goal can be taken up. If the goal is still unfulfilled and relevant, it is resumed, otherwise dropped. • T h e m a t i c c o h e r e n c e . A c o m p e t e n t agent

relates the topic of her contribution to what has been discussed previously or marks an awkward topic shift appropriately; otherwise the agent risks being understood. T h e m a t i c relatedness is based on the types of relation- ships which occur in the domain.

2.2 T h e C D M S y s t e m

T h e theoretical framework is formalised as an ap- proach to dialogue m a n a g e m e n t called the Con- structive Dialogue Model, CDM (Jokinen, 1994). In CDM, the dialogue is an instrument to ex- change new information on a particular topic to complete a real world task, and it is managed lo- cally by reacting to the changed dialogue context. T h e task division and information flow in a C D M system 2 is shown in Fig. 2. T h e dialogue manager operates on the Context Model which is a dynamic knowledge base containing facts a b o u t the agents' goals, expressive and evocative atti- tudes, central concepts (topic), and new informa- tion. It also has access to three static knowl- edge bases: Communicative Principles (knowl- edge a b o u t rational, cooperative communication), Application Model (knowledge a b o u t tasks and roles), and World Model (general knowledge a b o u t the entities and their relations in the world). 3

Dialogue contributions are constructed in three phases corresponding to the three main process- ing tasl~s. Analysis of the input message results in the user's communicative goal, and contains four subtasks: determine the explicitness level, in- t e r p r e t the propositional content, check coherence and verify obligations. Evaluation of the user goal concerns an appropriate joint purpose and deter- mines the next system goal. Response specifies the system's communicative goal up to the semantic representation using the same subtasks as analysis but in a reverse order. Evaluation and response form the agent's reaction.

2The prototype is implemented in SICStus Prolog 2.1, running under UNIX T M on a Sun SPARCStation. 3 Linguistic knowledge is encoded in a linguistic lex- icon and grammar, and not discussed here.

Input message

I Semantic Representation

I

ANALYSE ~ New tnfol icituess ~ User CGoal iosition

rence ~ations

User CGoal

LCT

~ATE purpose zation

System CG al ]

OND ~ations rence osition

Central Concept 1 User Attitudes

New- Info2 System CGoal

System Attitudes

C

O

N

T

E

X

T

M

O

D

E

L

icitness __, Central

I Semantic Representation Output message

Figure 2: Information flow in the C D M system.

T h e Context Model is represented as a parti- tioned Prolog database and the predicates have an extra a r g u m e n t referring to the contribution whose processing introduced them. In the a t t i t u d e language the predicates know, want and do repre- sent belief, intention and action, respectively, s refers to the system and u to the user. Communi- cative Principles are reasoning rules of the fort0:

if c n t x t F a c t l . . . . , c n t x t F a c t N

t h e n c n t x t F a c t M + 1 , . . . , c n t x t Y a c t K .

T h e World Model uses neo-Davidsonian event representation, and the Application Model provi- des mappings from World Model concepts to task and role related facts.

3 C o o p e r a t i v e G o a l F o r m u l a t i o n

[image:3.596.311.531.65.417.2]
(4)

goals fulfilled

unfulfilled

initiative speaker

partner

speaker

partner

central concept related

related unrelated related unrelated

unrelated

expected response f i n i s h / s t a r t finish/sp-eeify ~ p - n e w

n e w - r e q u e s t b a e k t o r e p e a t - n e w f o l l o w - u p - o l d n e w - q ~ - - -

nml-expected response l] c o n t i n u e / s t a r t

o b j e e ~ ~ g e l s e

n e w - i n d i r - r e q u e s t s u b q u e s t i o n

~]-eet

~ e d i

Figure 3: Possible joint purposes if the c o n t e x t u a l factors are assigned binary values.

reasoning rules are as follows (examples of the al- ternatives can be found in (aokinen, 1994)):

1. T h e a g e n t h a s f u l f i l l e d g o a l s o n l y , a n d t h e i n i t i a t i v e : Finish the dialogue or start a new one d e p e n d i n g on the p e n d i n g task goals

(finish/start/continue/obj ect/spe elf y).

Maintain the initiative if the response is re- lated, give the initiative if unrelated.

2. T h e a g e n t h a s f u l f i l l e d g o a l s o n l y , b u t n o i n i t i a t i v e : A d o p t the p a r t n e r ' s goal. Maintain the initiative if the response is ex- pected (follow-up-new,new-request), take

the initiative if the response is n o n - e x p e c t e d

(somethingelse ,new- indir-request).

3. T h e a g e n t h a s u n f n l f i l l e d g o a l s , a n d t h e i n i t i a t i v e : A d o p t the p a r t n e r ' s goal if the response is t h e m a t i c a l l y related ( b a c k t o , s u b q u e s t i o n ) , persist with the own goal if unrelated ( r e p e a t - n e w , o b j e c t ) . Maintain the initiative if the response is expected, give the initiative if non-expected.

4. T h e a g e n t h a s u n f u l f i l l e d g o a l s , b u t n o

initiative: A d o p t the p a r t n e r ' s goal. Main- tain the initiative if t h e response is t h e m a t - ically related ( f o l l o w - u p - o l d , c o n t i n u e ) , take the initiative if unrelated (new- q u e s t i o n , u o t r e l a t e d ) .

T h e joint purpose describes coinnmnieative in- tentions in a context where no speaker obligations or considerations hold. In order to a t t e n d the re- quirements of a particular c o m m u n i c a t i v e situa- tion, the joint p u r p o s e needs to be specified with respect to the a g e n t ' s role, task and c o m m u n i c a - tive obligations.

Specification of the joint p u r p o s e via t h e Ap- plication Model c a p t u r e s the cognitive consider= ation of Ideal C o o p e r a t i o n : the agent plans her response to be o p e r a t i o n a l l y a p p r o p r i a t e in the current situation. T h e result is a communicative goal (c-goal), a set of c o m m u n i c a t i v e intentions in- s t a n t i a t e d according to the c u r r e n t task and role. T h e c-goal is t h e n filtered t h r o u g h c o m m u n i c a - tive obligations which impleinent the ethical consi- deration of Ideal C o o p e r a t i o n : the a g e n t ' s com- municative c o m p e t e n c e shows in the ways she can realise the same c-goal in various situations. Some c o m m u n i c a t i v e obligations are listed in Fig. 4.

S i n c e r i t y : "do I know this or can provide evidence?"

1. Everything that the speaker asserts or implies is true unless otherwise explicitly stated.

M o t i v a t i o n : "can I say this?"

1. Everything that the speaker wants to know or wants the partner to do is motivated except if the speaker cmmot take the initiative on it. 2. Everything that addresses what the partner

wanted to know or wanted the speaker to do is motivated, except if the speaker emmot disclose the information or do the act.

3. Everything that is related to CC is motivated if not already known.

4. Everything that informs of inconsistency is mo- tivated if not already known.

C o n s i d e r a t i o n : "may I say this?"

1. If the partner's goal cammt be fulfilled (presup- positions are false, facts contradictory, no infor- mation exists), it is considerate to inform why (explain, compensate, initiate repair).

2. If the response would repeat previous informa- tion, it is considerate to leave this implicit unless the information is assigned a special emphasis. 3. If the partner's response is unrelated, it is con-

siderate to inform of the irrelevance, given that the speaker has unfulfilled goals.

4. [f the partner did not request a piece of related information, it is considerate to include this ex- plicitly in the response, given that the speaker intends to close the topic.

Figure 4: Some c o m m u n i c a t i v e obligations.

3.1 E x a m p l e

Consider the following sample dialogue where tile s y s t e m ' s task is to provide service information to the user:

U h I need a car.

$1: Do yon w a n t to b u y or rent one? Ui: Rent.

$2: W h e r e ? U3: In Bolton.

$3: OK. ttere are the car hire companies in Bolton: ....

T h e analysis of the first user c o n t r i b u t i o n U1 is given in Fig. 5. T h e c o n t e n t of the user's c-goal is inferred from the World Model which says t h a t 'needing a car' can be interpreted as ' w a n t i n g to

[image:4.596.106.473.61.158.2]
(5)

N E W I N P O : needE(n,u,c), user(u), car(c) U S E R C - G O A L : want(u, want(s, know(s,

[ w a n t ~ e E(h, u, e )]) )) C E N T R A L C O N C E P T : needE(n,u,c) E X P R E S S I V E A T T I T U D E S :

intention: user intend that system know P: want(u, know(s,[needE(n,u, c ), user(u), car(c)])) assumptions: user know that system not know P:

know(u, not know(s,[needE(n,u,d, user(u),car(e)])) E V O C A T I V E A T T I T U D E S :

intention: user intend that system intend that system know P:

want(u, want#, know(s,

[.eed E(n, u, c ), user(u ), ear(c)]))) want(u, want(s, know(s,[wantHave E(h,u,c )],

user(u ),~a,'(~ )]) ) )

Figure 5: Context Model after the user contribution 1 need a car. The constants n,u,c,h identify instanti- ated concepts.

In the beginning of the dialogue the system has no unfulfilled goals, and its role as an obedient in- formation provider does not allow it to have the initiative. Moreover, any contribution is trivially unrelated to the previous topic, since no previ- ous topic exists. According to the Joint Purpose rule (2), the user's c-goal is thus adopted, and the system also takes the initiative, since the user con- tribution is non-expected (an information seeker is expected to start with a question or a request). The joint purpose becomes new-indir-request with "user wants to have a car" as the content, i.e. the communicative strategy is to share the user's want to have a car, and check if this want can be satis- fied within the Application Model.

The system cannot provide the user with a car, but it can provide information about the services that, enable the user to have a car. Application Model lists car hire companies and car garages as possible services, so the communicative goal is for- mulated as to know which is the preferred service. The services are associated with renting or buying cars, thus the disjunction is realised as 5'1.

The system responses $2 and 5"3 are based on the same strategy baclcto: the system 'goes back' to adopt the user's previous unfulfilled goal and tries to satisfy this in the u p d a t e d context. 4 How- ever, they carry different c-goals due to different specification in the Application Model: $2 aims at narrowing down the database search, 5,3 com- pletes the original task. Finally, the communica- tive obligation Consideration (4) requires t h a t the application service (car hire c o m p a n y ) and loca- tion (Bolton) are explicitly expressed in $3 before the list of services.

4The user response fulfills expectations and is the- maritally related, and the system has the initiative and unfulfilled goals, at least one based on the origi- nal task to provide information.

4 D i s c u s s i o n a n d r e l a t e d w o r k

In Section 1 we pointed out three i m p o r t a n t as- pects of dialogues which have been insufficiently accounted for in the earlier approaches to dialogue m a n a g e m e n t . In CDM, these aspects form the ba- sis of the s y s t e m ' s functionality: dialogues are re- garded as collaborative activities, planned locally in the changed context as reactions to the previ- ous contributions and governed by the rationality principles of Ideal Cooperation. The logical omni- science assumption is tackled by partitioning the C o n t e x t Model and focussing on specific knowl- edge with the hel f ) of thematic coherence; also ra- tionality considerations constrain reasoning.

By adhering to general communicative princi- ples, CDM provides a new and uniform way to treat various p h e n o m e n a that have been sepa- rately studied in previous research: goal formula- tion, coherence and cooperativeness. Communica- tive principles fimetion on the following levels:

1. D e t e r m i n a t i o n o f t h e j o i n t p u r p o s e : reasoning a b o u t a communicative strategy in the context (expectations, initiatives, unflfl- filled goals, thematic coherence)

2. S e l e c t i o n o f t h e c o m m u n i c a t i v e goal: filtering the joint purpose with respect to the agent's role and task.

3. R e a l i s a t i o n o f t h e goal: specifying the goal in regard to the communicative obliga- tions sincerity, motivation and consideration.

However, we also use insights from the huge body of research t h a t exists on dialogue man- agement and natural language planning. For in- stance, the negotiative nature of dialogues is em- phasised in (Moore and Paris, 1993) who show how rhetorical knowledge can be combined with the knowledge a b o u t the speaker's intentions and communicative goals so t h a t the system can un- derstand follow-up questions or justify its expla- nations. Our work differs from this in t h a t we study general requirements of communication rather than rhetorical relations and their augmen- tation with speaker intentions, to determine @- propriate responses. It is possible to modify our joint purpose algorithm with information a b o u t rhetorical relations so as to check expectations in regard to argmnentation, or to include rhetorical knowledge in the obligations used when reason- ing a b o u t multisentential contributions, but as our p r i m a r y goal has been to specify communicative principles and use t h e m in the formalisation of the cooperative and rational nature of dialogues, this kind of extension is left for future.

[image:5.596.79.298.61.224.2]
(6)

completion of the proof the agent is set to do, the agent a t t e m p t s to provide the missing axioms through interaction. This is similar to our basic as- sumption of how domain tasks give rise to eonlinu- nication. T h e differences lie again in our einphasis on 'Rational and Cooperative C o m m u n i c a t i o n ' as opposed to 'Interaction as a FMlure to Prove'.

In abandoning dialogue g r a m m a r and speech act classification, we agree with the common view currently held among researches: dialogue struc- ture is constructed on the basis of the partici- pants' beliefs and intentions, and speech act types are at most convenient abbreviations for a set of attitudes held by the speakers, but do not constitute an explanation of the dialogue (Co- hen and Levesque, 1990; Galliers, 1989). We Mso use contextual knowledge extensively, and connect intention-based approaches to practical dialogue management: rationality and cooperation are not only tied to the agent's beliefs and intentions of the desidered next state of the world, but also to the wider social context in which the communica- tion takes place.

5 C o n c l u s i o n a n d f u t u r e d i r e c t i o n s

This paper has presented a new way to formulate system goals in intelligent dialogue systems. It ad- vocates a view-point where the system's fnnction- ality is iml)roved by relating the dialogue situation to communication in general. The constraints of rational, cooperative communication p,:ovide the framework in which to deal with contributions: communicators have a joint purpose, they obey communicative obligations and they trust t h a t the p a r t n e r behaves so t h a t these constraints are tiff- filled. Dialogues are dynamic constructions, and contributions are locally planned and realised so t h a t the communicative requirements of the dia- logue us a whole are respected.

Current interests concern the extension of the communicative principles into different activities and agent roles. This contributes to the generality of the model by spelling out specific requirements of different communicative situations. It also en- ables us to study strategic planning and how dif- ferent roles affect the obligations t h a t the agents want to obey (e.g. in conflict situations). Work is now in progress to cover other types of task dia- logues, and to enhance the impleinentation.

R e f e r e n c e s

J. F. Allen and C. R. Perrault. 1980. Analyzing intention in utterances. Artificial Intelligence, 15:143 178.

J. Allwood. 1976. Linguistic Communication as Action an, d Cooperation. D e p a r t m e n t of Lin- guistics, University of GSteborg. G o t h e n b u r g Monographs in Linguistics 2.

J. Allwood. 1992. On dialogue cohesion. Tech- nical Report G o t h e n b u r g Papers in Theoretical Linguistics 65, University of Gothenburg.

D. Appelt. 1985. Planning Natural Language Ut- terances. Cambridge University Press, Cain- bridge.

E. Bilange. 1992. Dialogue personne- machine. Mod~lisation et r&disation informa- tiquc. Ilerm~s, Paris.

It. C. Bunt, R. J. Beun, F. J. H. Dols, J. A. van der Linden, and G. O. thoe Sehwartzenl)erg. 1984. T h e T E N D U M dialogue system and its theo retical basis. Technical R e p o r t 19, IPO. A. Cawsey. 1993. Explanation and Interaction.

~lT~e Computer Generation of Explanatory Dia- logues. The M I T Press, Cambridge, MA. C. Cherniak. 1986. Minimal Rationality. The

M1T Press. Cambridge, MA.

it. It. Clark and D. Wilkes-Gibbs. 1990. Refer- ring as a collaborative process. In P. R. Cohen, J. Morgan, and M. E. Pollack, editors, Inten- tions In Communication, pages 463-493. The M I T Press. Cambridge, MA.

P. R. Cohen and It. J. Levesque. 1990. Rational interaction as the basis for communication. In P. R. Cohen, J. Morgan, and M. E. Pollack, ed- itors, Intentions in Communication, pages 221 255. The M I T Press. Cambridge, MA.

J. R. Galliers. 1989. A theoreticM framework for c o m p u t e r models of cooperative diMogue, acknowledging multi-agent conflict. Technical Report 172, University of Carat)ridge.

B. a. Grosz and C. L. Sidner. 1986. Attention, in- tentions, and the structure of discourse. Com- putational Linguistics, 12(3): 175-204.

C. I. Guinn. 1994. Mcta-Dialogue Behaviors: Im- proving the Efficiency of Human-Machine Dia- logue. A Computational Model of Variable Ini- tiative and Negotiation in Collaborative Prob- lem-Solving. Ph.D. thesis, Duke University. K. Jokinen. 1994. Response Planning in Informa-

tion-Seeking Dialogues. Ph.D. thesis, UMIST. K. Jokinen. 1995. Rational agency. In M. Fehling,

editor, Rational Agency: Concepts, Theories, Models, and Applications, pages 89--93. Pro- ceedings of The AAAI-95 Fall Symposium.

J. D. Moore and C. L. Paris. 1993. Planning text for advisory diMogues: Capturing inten- tional and rhetorical information. Computa- tional Linguistics, 19(4):651-694.

J. Nivre. (Ed.)(1992). Corpus collection an'd anal- ysis. Technical Report D2.1, PLUS deliverable.

Figure

Figure 2: Information flow in the CDM system.
Figure 3: Possible joint purposes if the contextual factors are assigned binary values
Figure 5: Context Model after the user contribution 1 need a car. The constants n,u,c,h identify instanti- ated concepts

References

Related documents

A FLUID FORMULATION OF THE BANDWIDTH SHARING SCHEME In order to assess the performance gain achievable by the proposed scheme, we consider a distributed upload scenario in which

Learning about the differences between primary and secondary care will help the specialty trainee gain a broader understanding of the principles and practice of

Energy efficiency also delivers social and economic benefits Improved energy efficiency Employment impacts Asset values • Property prices • Productivity Consumer surplus

 The intermediate storage solution and export abroad should have been described as alternatives to a permanent repository..  No description of the sustainability of the

A range o f ligands and their corresponding metal complexes have been screened for their ability to catalytically reduce silver ions in a Timm’s type

Sprchové dvere TWIST krídlové s pevným segmentom pre bočnú stenu / rohový vstup, profily strieborná lesk + doplnky chróm, sklo číre antiplaque, ĽAVÉ / PRAVÉ (montáž:

In this advocacy project, an occupational therapy doctoral student intern explored occupational therapy’s role on the diabetes management team at a large medical center in

Interpersonal, dependent stress will mediate the relationship between co- rumination and depressive symptoms prospectively more strongly compared to interpersonal, independent